The American Psychiatric Association (APA) has updated its Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including with new information specifically addressed to individuals in the European Economic Area. As described in the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, this website utilizes cookies, including for the purpose of offering an optimal online experience and services tailored to your preferences.

Please read the entire Privacy Policy and Terms of Use. By closing this message, browsing this website, continuing the navigation, or otherwise continuing to use the APA's websites, you confirm that you understand and accept the terms of the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including the utilization of cookies.

×
Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ps.201300107

Abstract

Objective

Individuals with serious mental illnesses are very likely to interact with police officers. The crisis intervention team (CIT) model is being widely implemented by police departments across the United States to improve officers’ responses. However, little research exists on officer-level outcomes. The authors compared officers with or without CIT training on six key constructs related to the CIT model: knowledge about mental illnesses, attitudes about serious mental illnesses and treatments, self-efficacy for deescalating crisis situations and making referrals to mental health services, stigmatizing attitudes, deescalation skills, and referral decisions.

Methods

The sample included 586 officers, 251 of whom had received the 40-hour CIT training (median of 22 months before the study), from six police departments in Georgia. In-depth, in-person assessments of officers’ knowledge, attitudes, and skills were administered. Many measures were linked to two vignettes, in written and video formats, depicting typical police encounters with individuals with psychosis or with suicidality.

Results

CIT-trained officers had consistently better scores on knowledge, diverse attitudes about mental illnesses and their treatments, self-efficacy for interacting with someone with psychosis or suicidality, social distance stigma, deescalation skills, and referral decisions. Effect sizes for some measures, including deescalation skills and referral decisions pertaining to psychosis, were substantial (d=.71 and .57, respectively, p<.001).

Conclusions

CIT training of police officers resulted in sizable and persisting improvements in diverse aspects of knowledge, attitudes, and skills. Research should now address potential outcomes at the system level and for individuals with whom officers interact.

Police officers are often first responders to emergency calls involving individuals with serious mental illnesses (1), defined as mental disorders that substantially interfere with a person’s life activities and ability to function, such as schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, and major depression. In fact, up to 10% of all police contacts involve a person with a mental illness (2), and officers provide up to one-third of all emergency mental health referrals (3). Thus, because officers are gatekeepers not only to the justice system but also to the psychiatric system (4), they serve as de facto mental health professionals (5), making decisions about whether to refer a person to mental health services or to arrest and incarcerate the person, as well as other discretionary decisions. Despite the magnitude of these decisions, which involve the most vulnerable individuals with serious mental illnesses and some of the most strained public sectors, officers usually receive little training about mental illnesses, although they want more training and find the topic very important to their work (6).

To improve officers’ responses to individuals with serious mental illnesses, the crisis intervention team (CIT) model was developed in 1988 in Memphis (79). CIT provides certain officers with 40 hours of specialized training by police trainers, local mental health professionals, family advocates, and consumer groups (7,10). The training equips officers with knowledge, attitudes, and skills to enhance their responses to persons with serious mental illnesses or to those experiencing a psychiatric crisis (1,2,8). After training, officers are specialized first-line responders to such calls (1114). CIT also supports partnerships between psychiatric emergency services and police departments, encouraging treatment rather than jail when appropriate (1,10). Thus, in addition to its other goals (for example, improved officer and citizen safety), CIT is a form of prebooking jail diversion.

It is estimated that more than 2,700 police departments in the United States have implemented CIT (personal communication, Dupont R, July 2013). Given the very wide implementation and rapid growth of CIT in recent years, research on this police-based collaboration between the law enforcement, advocacy, and mental health systems is urgently needed (15). This study focused on how CIT training affected a number of key officer-level outcomes that likely underlie its broader beneficial effects. The purpose of this study was to document differences between officers with and without CIT training across six key constructs: knowledge about mental illnesses, attitudes about serious mental illnesses and their treatments, self-efficacy for deescalating crisis situations and making referrals to mental health services, stigmatizing attitudes toward persons with serious mental illnesses, deescalation skills, and referral decisions. We first assessed potentially important covariates by determining differences between the two groups in demographic characteristics, experience, and empathy. We then determined differences between the groups in regard to the six constructs of interest, taking into account the effects of covariates.

Methods

Participants

Police officers (N=586), including both CIT-trained (N=251) and traditional officers without CIT training (N=335), were recruited from six police departments in Georgia. As described in a companion article (16), each department had implemented CIT training of officers with local instructors and a standardized 40-hour curriculum, which was developed and made available through a statewide CIT initiative (14). Self-selection (volunteering) for CIT specialization is commonly considered a core element of the CIT model. The percentage of participating CIT officers who reported having volunteered for CIT training (rather than having been assigned to it) ranged from 36% to 100% across the six departments (N=171, or 68% of the 251 CIT officers).

After hearing about the study through roll-call presentations, e-mail notices, flyers posted in department precincts, or word of mouth, officers with or without CIT training who were interested in participating called the research team to register for one of 34 proctored, group-based, in-depth survey administrations between April and October 2010. Between six and 29 officers participated in the survey groups. Officers took part during off-duty hours and were compensated to remunerate them for travel time to and from the assessment, approximately three hours of survey participation, and parking.

The mean±SD age of the 586 officers was 37.0±8.7 years. Participants had been officers for an average of 10.0±7.7 years. Nineteen percent of participants (N=114) were women. Sixteen percent (N=95) were high school graduates, 40% (N=237) had completed some college, 10% (N=58) had an associate’s degree, 26% (N=150) had a bachelor’s degree, 6% (N=34) had a master’s degree, and 2% (N=12) did not specify. Thirty-five percent (N=203) self-identified as African American; 59% (N=347) as Caucasian and non-Hispanic; 3% (N=20) as Hispanic; and 1% each as Native American or Pacific Islander (N=8), Asian (N=4), or other or did not specify (N=4). Among the 251 CIT-trained officers, time since training varied from less than one month to more than seven years (median months since training was 22). For half of the CIT-trained officers (N=126), time since training was between seven and 36 months. For subsequent analyses, participants were coded 1–5 for the first to fifth quintiles of time since training.

Procedures and measures

Survey administration required approximately three hours. About a third of the survey focused on demographic characteristics, experience, empathy, knowledge, and attitudinal factors. The remainder focused on attitudinal and behavioral responses to two vignettes, one written and one video, which were developed (the videos were professionally produced) specifically for this study. Groups of officers received one vignette in a video format and the other as a written script, in a counterbalanced manner. One vignette (4.2-minute video) depicted an agitated, disheveled, disorganized, and psychotic man digging through a trashcan outside a business establishment, with an officer arriving on the scene (herein called the “psychosis vignette”). The other vignette (2.5-minute video) presented an intoxicated and suicidal woman who was distraught because of a relationship break-up and who had locked herself in her home bathroom, with an officer arriving on the scene (the “suicidality vignette”). The study was approved by the Emory University Institutional Review Board, and participants provided written informed consent.

With regard to the in-depth assessment, all constructs were scored as the mean of items answered if responses were given for at least 75% of items, except for experience with mental health treatment and knowledge about mental illnesses. Reverse scoring was conducted as appropriate, so that higher scores for all measures represent more of the named attribute.

The first portion of the assessment included a number of measures not linked to vignettes. To assess experience with mental health treatment, three items asked whether the participant (“self”), a family member, or a friend had received or was now receiving mental health treatment and a fourth items asked whether the participant, a family member, or friend had volunteered or worked in the mental health field (“other”). We created an experience index, coded 0–5, to summarize these four items: 0 if the participant responded negatively to all four items (N=193, 33%), 1 for an affirmative response only for “other” (N=64, 11%), 2 for an affirmative response for a friend but not for a family member or “self” (N=94, 16%), 3 for an affirmative response for a family member but not for friend or “self” (N=59, 10%), 4 for an affirmative response for both a family member and a friend (N=94, 16%), and 5 for an affirmative response for “self” (N=82, 14%).

The construct of empathy toward individuals with mental illnesses, which served as a potential personality-related covariate, was assessed with an adapted version of a nine-item measure (17). Respondents are asked to “indicate how much you feel each emotion toward people with mental illnesses”; each item (for example, compassion, disgust, and respect) is rated 0, not at all, to 10, extremely (Cronbach’s α=.78). To measure knowledge about mental illnesses, officers completed the 33-item Knowledge of Mental Illnesses Test (18), scored as the percentage of correct items.

Several measures were administered to thoroughly assess the construct labeled attitudes about mental illnesses and their treatments. The Opinions About Mental Illnesses Scale (19,20) consists of five subscales: authoritarianism (scored such that high scores indicate less authoritarianism), benevolence, mental hygiene, social restrictiveness, and interpersonal etiology (Cronbach’s α=.69, .63, .42, .72, and .75, respectively). Two additional scales assessed attitudes about community mental health treatment facilities (21,22) and attitudes about psychiatric treatments more broadly, in addition to hospitals and community facilities (23) (Cronbach’s α=.83, and .72). The six reliable scales (excluding mental hygiene) were intercorrelated (mean r=.57, range=.29–.67). Accordingly, an “opinions about mental illnesses” variable was computed as the mean of these six scales (items for all scales were rated 1–6) (Cronbach’s α=.84).

All remaining measures were administered twice, linked to the two vignettes. Two rating scales pertained to the attitudes construct. The Attribution Questionnaire (24,25) consists of 21 items in six domains (for example, personal responsibility, pity, and anger). The 12-item Revised Causal Dimensions Scale (2628) assesses causal attributions along four domains: external control, personal control, locus of causality-internality, and stability. The latter two domains had unacceptably low internal consistency and were not considered further. The mean Cronbach’s α for these eight attitudinal domains was .76 when linked to the psychosis vignette (range=.59–.87) and .78 with respect to the suicidality vignette (range=.62–.87).

The construct of self-efficacy for deescalating crisis situations and making referrals to mental health services was measured with a 16-item questionnaire that was rated on a 4-point scale, ranging from 1, not at all confident, to 4, very confident (23) (Cronbach’s α=.94 when linked to both vignettes). To measure the construct of stigma toward people with mental illnesses, we used two instruments—an adapted version of the Social Distance Scale and a semantic differential measure. On the former, participants rated their willingness to be close to (for example, live next door to) the individual depicted in the vignette on a 4-point scale, ranging from 1, very willing, to 4, very unwilling (23) (Cronbach’s α=.92 when linked to both vignettes). For the second stigma measure, respondents rated an average person, the man in the psychosis vignette, and the woman in the suicidality vignette on 12 semantic differentials (for example, valuable/worthless) using a rating scale from 1 to 7. The 12 items were scored so that higher values reflected more positive judgments (Cronbach’s α=.86, .83, and .84 for the three persons rated, respectively). A score reflecting total stigmatizing attitudes toward the man with psychosis was computed by subtracting the mean score on the psychosis vignette from the mean score for the average person; the same method was used for the suicidal woman. To make all values positive, 4 was added to each score, resulting in an index varying from just above 0 to just below 9.

Finally, the two constructs of deescalation skills and referral decisions were measured by two instruments designed specifically for this study and tested previously in an independent sample of nearly 200 officers (23). Both were eight-item instruments assessing officers’ opinions about the effectiveness of specific actions in the two situations depicted; responses were rated on a 4-point scale ranging from 1, very negative, to 4, very positive.

Statistical analysis

Because of the extent of the data deriving from these multiple measures, the large sample size, and the number of analyses, we used p≤.01 as the criterion for significance; effects significant at the .05 but not at the .01 level are referred to as marginal. Throughout, we present effect sizes as well as statistical significance (29). We used Cohen’s d, the standardized difference between two means (30), following Cohen’s criteria: .2 is a small (weak) effect, .5 is a medium (moderate) effect, and .8 is a large (strong) effect.

Results

Differences in characteristics, experience, empathy

Officers with and without CIT training did not differ in age, race, years of education, or years of service as an officer. The proportion of women was about twice as high in the CIT-trained group (N=67, 27%, versus N=47, 14%; odds ratio [OR]=2.23, p<.001). The groups also differed on the two additional potential covariates. Specifically, the mean scores for experience and empathy were higher for CIT-trained officers than for those without CIT training (experience, 2.4 versus 1.8 on a 0–5 scale; t=4.23, df=584, d=.35, p<.001; empathy, 6.8 versus 6.3 on a 0–10 scale; t=3.88, df=583, d=.33, p<.001).

Group differences in the six key constructs

Group differences were examined with analyses of covariance that included age, gender, years served as an officer, years of education, the experience index, and empathy as covariates. Controlling for these covariates did not substantially change the findings of between-group differences that were found with t tests. The CIT-trained group differed consistently from the group without CIT training (Table 1). For example, the former group scored higher on knowledge and opinions about mental illnesses and lower on anger and fear attitudes.

Table 1 Measures of six key constructs among officers with or without crisis intervention team (CIT) training
VariableaPossiblescorerangeWith CIT training
Without CIT training
tdfpdb
NScore
NScore
MSDMSD
Knowledge about mental illnesses 0–100251591533554154.32584<.001.36
Attitudes about mental illnesses and their treatments
 Opinions About Mental Illnesses Scale1–62494.24.453314.01.406.44578<.001.54
 Attribution Questionnaire
  Personal responsibility (P)1–92492.541.303322.961.49–3.48579.001–.29
  Personal responsibility (S)1–92514.891.813335.311.74–2.88582.004–.24
  Pity (P)1–92496.351.723315.711.824.23578<.001.36
  Pity (S)1–92505.911.743335.571.782.28581.023.19
  Anger (P)1–92493.291.733313.581.74–2.02578.043–.17
  Anger (S)1–92513.131.663333.541.79–2.80582.005–.23
  Fear (P)1–92494.711.733315.271.80–3.76578<.001–.32
  Fear (S)1–92514.251.633334.551.63–2.16582.031–.18
  Help (P)1–92503.481.393312.891.205.46579<.001.46
  Help (S)1–92514.991.803334.621.692.52582.012.21
  Coercion-segregation (P)1–92494.771.763325.621.64–6.02579<.001–.51
  Coercion-segregation (S)1–92513.061.563333.421.58–2.73582.006–.23
 Revised Causal Dimensions Scale
  External control (P)1–92504.591.693324.481.55.76580.45.06
  External control (S)1–92514.731.643334.601.63.90582.37.08
  Personal control (P)1–92503.501.613323.641.71–1.04580.30–.09
  Personal control (S)1–92515.991.773336.141.79–1.00582.32–.08
Self-efficacy
 Self-efficacy (P)1–42503.34.443323.05.467.68580<.001.64
 Self-efficacy (S)1–42513.46.423333.31.404.32582<.001.36
Stigma
 Social distance (P)1–42502.43.673312.72.65–5.20579<.001–.44
 Social distance (S)1–42512.09.683332.29.65–3.66582<.001–.31
 Stigmatizing attitudes (P)0–92504.82.843304.92.93–1.34578.18–.11
 Stigmatizing attitudes (S)0–92444.65.923274.63.96.26569.80.02
Deescalation skills
 Deescalation skills (P)1–42493.20.363322.97.318.45579<.001.71
 Deescalation skills (S)1–42513.18.323333.05.314.92582<.001.41
Referral decisions
 Referral decisions (P)1–42493.46.373323.24.396.78579<.001.57
 Referral decisions (S)1–42513.49.363333.33.375.30582<.001.44

a P, psychosis vignette; S, suicidality vignette

b Cohen’s d is the standardized difference between the means.

Table 1 Measures of six key constructs among officers with or without crisis intervention team (CIT) training
Enlarge table

Results for the 17 items constituting the attitudes construct (the first of which is a mean of six scales) are shown in Table 1. Except for external and personal control items, differences were at least marginal for the remaining 13 items and significant (p<.01) for nine. Among these nine items, the effect size was weak for seven and moderate for two (opinions about mental illnesses and coercion-segregation pertaining to psychosis). The consistent pattern of differences was not attributable to strong correlations among variables; the mean absolute correlation between variables for both the psychosis and suicidality vignettes was .16. Mean differences were greater for the psychosis vignette compared with the suicidality vignette for five of six corresponding pairs (anger was the exception). Figure 1 displays absolute effect sizes for differences between CIT-trained officers and those without CIT training.

Figure 1 Effect sizes for differences between officers with or without crisis intervention team training in responses to key construct measuresa

a Effect sizes for knowledge and opinions about mental illnesses (top two bars) were not linked to the vignettes used in the assessment. Other bars pertain to variables linked to the psychosis or suicidality vignette. Only ten of the 13 vignette-linked variables that significantly differentiated the groups are shown. Effect sizes between .20 and .50 (vertical lines) are regarded as weak, and those between .50 and .80 as moderate.

Regarding between-group differences for other key variables, differences were significant (p<.001) for all except the stigmatizing attitudes scores derived from the semantic differential scales. For the variables that showed a significant difference, the effect size was weak for five and moderate for three (self-efficacy, deescalation skills, and referral decisions linked to the psychosis vignette). All correlations between corresponding items across vignettes were strong (mean r=.64; range=.57–.71), although mean differences were greater for the psychosis vignette than the suicidality vignette.

Quintile of time since training was largely unassociated with the variables listed in Table 1. Of 28 correlations, only the two involving deescalation skills (for both vignettes) were significant (for the psychosis vignette, r=.19, p=.003; for the suicidality vignette, r=.18, p=.005). Specifically, for the psychosis and suicidality vignettes, mean deescalation skills scores increased monotonically for the first through fifth quintiles of time since training, from 3.12 to 3.35 for the psychosis vignette and from 3.10 to 3.26 for the suicidality vignette.

Discussion

Even when the analyses controlled for covariates such as years of education, personal and family experience with mental health treatment, and empathy, CIT-trained officers had consistently better scores than officers without CIT training on knowledge, diverse attitudes toward serious mental illnesses and their treatments, self-efficacy, social distance stigma, deescalation skills, and referral decisions. Effect sizes for some of these—including self-efficacy, deescalation skills, and referral decisions pertaining to psychosis, which are arguably most central to the problems that CIT training seeks to address—were in the moderate range. Notably, given that officers had completed CIT training a median of 22 months before the research assessment, these findings are particularly impressive and confirm that previously reported improvements in knowledge, attitudes, stigma, and self-efficacy immediately after training (31,32) do, in fact, persist.

These results suggest that CIT is effective at the officer level. However, the more difficult task is to address the immediate, short-term, and perhaps even long-term outcomes of the individuals with whom officers interact, including improved safety and less use of force, fewer arrests (that is, prebooking jail diversion), enhanced case finding and referral, and improved mental health and criminal justice outcomes. It is also important to address the system-level effects of CIT (such as criminal justice cost savings).

Although each of our six key constructs is meaningful to CIT, deescalation skills are of particular importance because the “criminalization” of mental illnesses may be prominently related to impulsivity or emotionally motivated responses to perceived provocation (33), rather than to untreated symptoms alone (34). Thus deescalation may be critical to advancing jail diversion. Enhanced referral decisions, when joined with improvements in mental health services, represent a crucial officer-level outcome of CIT training. This is especially important with respect to arguments that “criminalization” inappropriately blames officers, many of whom use arrest and detention as a “mercy booking” in an attempt to provide individuals with mental health services in jail because of the perceived unavailability or ineffectiveness of the mental health system (35).

We acknowledge several methodological limitations. First, all CIT-trained officers were from a single state, which relies on a relatively standardized CIT curriculum. However, Georgia’s CIT program is guided by the core elements of the CIT model (36), suggesting that results may be broadly generalizable. Second, whether enhanced knowledge and more positive attitudes toward people with serious mental illnesses affect encounter resolutions remains unknown. Self-report of deescalation skills and referral decisions is clearly only a proxy for actual behaviors during an interaction. Although we linked most of our measures to contextualized, realistic vignettes to optimize validity, we do not know whether the perceived enhanced deescalation skills translate into safer resolutions of crises in the field.

Conclusions

Improving police responses to persons with serious mental illnesses is now a national priority in the law enforcement and criminal justice sectors (37), as well as in the mental health community. CIT is a police-based approach to addressing this priority—one that is supported by mental health and advocacy groups. The use of mental health courts has recently been shown to be effective in terms of lower rearrest rates and fewer incarceration days (38). However, CIT represents a prebooking approach that may also have an impact on these and other outcomes. The findings reported here demonstrate that CIT training of police officers results in substantial and persisting improvements in officers’ knowledge, attitudes, and skills. Research should also address other outcomes that may accompany our documented officer-level findings, especially safer outcomes for both citizens and officers (for example, less agitation and reduced use of force) and more appropriate dispositions in terms of both reduced arrests (that is, prebooking jail diversion) and enhanced case-finding and referral to mental health services, which are topics of the companion article (16). Such research would determine whether CIT is an effective mental health service augmentation beyond its now proven beneficial effects for officers.

Dr. Compton and Ms. Broussard are with the Department of Psychiatry, Lenox Hill Hospital, the North Shore–LIJ Health System, New York City (e-mail: ). When this study was conducted, they were with Emory University, Atlanta, and The George Washington University, Washington, D.C. Dr. Bakeman is with the Department of Psychology, Georgia State University, Atlanta. Ms. Hankerson-Dyson, Ms. Husbands, Ms. Krishan, Ms. Stewart-Hutto, and Dr. D’Orio are with the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Emory University School of Medicine, Atlanta. Dr. Oliva was formerly with the Georgia Bureau of Investigation, Atlanta, and is now retired. Dr. Thompson is with the Department of Behavioral Sciences and Health Education, Rollins School of Public Health, Emory University, Atlanta. Dr. Watson is with the Jane Addams College of Social Work, University of Illinois at Chicago.

Acknowledgments and disclosures

The research reported in this article was supported by the National Institute of Mental Health under award number R01 MH082813 to Dr. Compton. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health.

The authors report no competing interests.

References

1 Lamb HR, Weinberger LE, DeCuir WJ: The police and mental health. Psychiatric Services 53:1266–1271, 2002LinkGoogle Scholar

2 Deane MW, Steadman HJ, Borum R, et al.: Emerging partnerships between mental health and law enforcement. Psychiatric Services 50:99–101, 1999LinkGoogle Scholar

3 Borum R, Deane MW, Steadman HJ, et al.: Police perspectives on responding to mentally ill people in crisis: perceptions of program effectiveness. Behavioral Sciences and the Law 16:393–405, 1998Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

4 Wells W, Schafer JA: Officer perceptions of police responses to persons with a mental illness. Policing 29:578–601, 2006CrossrefGoogle Scholar

5 Teplin LA, Pruett NS: Police as streetcorner psychiatrist: managing the mentally ill. International Journal of Law and Psychiatry 15:139–156, 1992Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

6 Vermette HS, Pinals DA, Appelbaum PS: Mental health training for law enforcement professionals. Journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law 33:42–46, 2005MedlineGoogle Scholar

7 Dupont R, Cochran S: Police response to mental health emergencies: barriers to change. Journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law 28:338–344, 2000MedlineGoogle Scholar

8 Steadman HJ, Deane MW, Borum R, et al.: Comparing outcomes of major models of police responses to mental health emergencies. Psychiatric Services 51:645–649, 2000LinkGoogle Scholar

9 Compton MT, Broussard B, Munetz M, et al.: The Crisis Intervention Team (CIT) Model of Collaboration Between Law Enforcement and Mental Health. New York, Novinka/Nova Science Publishers, 2011Google Scholar

10 Cochran S, Deane MW, Borum R: Improving police response to mentally ill people. Psychiatric Services 51:1315–1316, 2000LinkGoogle Scholar

11 Bower DL, Pettit G: The Albuquerque police department’s crisis intervention team: a report card. FBI Law Enforcement Bulletin 70:1–6, 2001Google Scholar

12 Hails J, Borum R: Police training and specialized approaches to respond to people with mental illnesses. Crime and Delinquency 49:52–61, 2003CrossrefGoogle Scholar

13 Oliva JR, Haynes N, Covington DW, et al.: Crisis intervention team (CIT) programs; in Responding to Individuals With Mental Illnesses. Edited by Compton MTKotwicki RJ. Sudbury, Mass, Jones and Bartlett, 2006Google Scholar

14 Oliva JR, Compton MT: A statewide Crisis Intervention Team (CIT) initiative: evolution of the Georgia CIT program. Journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law 36:38–46, 2008MedlineGoogle Scholar

15 Compton MT, Bahora M, Watson AC, et al.: A comprehensive review of extant research on Crisis Intervention Team (CIT) programs. Journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law 36:47–55, 2008MedlineGoogle Scholar

16 Compton MT, Bakeman R, Broussard B, et al.: The police-based crisis intervention team (CIT) model: II. effects on level of force and resolution, referral, and arrest. Psychiatric Services, 2013; doi 10.1176/appi.ps.201300108Google Scholar

17 Levy SR, Freitas AL, Salovey P: Construing action abstractly and blurring social distinctions: implications for perceiving homogeneity among, but also empathizing with and helping, others. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 83:1224–1238, 2002Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

18 Compton MT, Hankerson-Dyson D, Broussard B: Development, item analysis, and initial reliability and validity of a multiple-choice knowledge of mental illnesses test for lay samples. Psychiatry Research 189:141–148, 2011Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

19 Cohen J, Struening EL: Opinions about mental illness in the personnel of two large mental hospitals. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology 64:349–360, 1962Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

20 Struening EL, Cohen J: Factorial invariance and other psychometric characteristics of five opinions about mental illness factors. Educational and Psychological Measurement 23:289–298, 1963CrossrefGoogle Scholar

21 Taylor SM, Dear MJ, Hall GB: Attitudes toward the mentally ill and reactions to mental health facilities. Social Science and Medicine 13D:281–290, 1979Google Scholar

22 Taylor SM, Dear MJ: Scaling community attitudes toward the mentally ill. Schizophrenia Bulletin 7:225–240, 1981Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

23 Broussard B, Krishan S, Hankerson-Dyson D, et al.: Development and initial reliability and validity of four self-report measures used in research on interactions between police officers and individuals with mental illnesses. Psychiatry Research 189:458–462, 2011Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

24 Corrigan PW, River LP, Lundin RK, et al.: Predictors of participation in campaigns against mental illness stigma. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 187:378–380, 1999Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

25 Corrigan PW, Markowitz FE, Watson A, et al.: An attribution model of public discrimination towards persons with mental illness. Journal of Health and Social Behavior 44:162–179, 2003Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

26 Russell D: The Causal Dimension Scale: a measure of how individuals perceive causes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 42:1137–1145, 1982CrossrefGoogle Scholar

27 Russell DW, McAuley E, Tarico V: Measuring causal attributions for success and failure: a comparison of methodologies for assessing causal dimensions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 52:1248–1257, 1987CrossrefGoogle Scholar

28 McAuley E, Duncan TE, Russell DW: Measuring causal attributions: the Revised Causal Dimension Scale (CDSII). Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 18:566–573, 1992CrossrefGoogle Scholar

29 Wilkinson L: Statistical methods in psychology journals: guidelines and explanations. American Psychologist 54:594–604, 1999CrossrefGoogle Scholar

30 Cohen J: Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences, 2nd ed. Hillsdale, NJ, Erlbaum, 1988Google Scholar

31 Compton MT, Esterberg ML, McGee R, et al.: Crisis intervention team training: changes in knowledge, attitudes, and stigma related to schizophrenia. Psychiatric Services 57:1199–1202, 2006LinkGoogle Scholar

32 Bahora M, Hanafi S, Chien VH, et al.: Preliminary evidence of effects of crisis intervention team training on self-efficacy and social distance. Administration and Policy in Mental Health and Mental Health Services Research 35:159–167, 2008Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

33 Junginger J, Claypoole K, Laygo R, et al.: Effects of serious mental illness and substance abuse on criminal offenses. Psychiatric Services 57:879–882, 2006LinkGoogle Scholar

34 Peterson J, Skeem JL, Hart E, et al.: Analyzing offense patterns as a function of mental illness to test the criminalization hypothesis. Psychiatric Services 61:1217–1222, 2010LinkGoogle Scholar

35 Fisher WH, Grudzinskas AJ: Crisis intervention teams as the solution to managing crises involving persons with serious psychiatric illnesses: does one size fit all? Journal of Police Crisis Negotiations 10:58–71, 2010CrossrefGoogle Scholar

36 Dupont R, Cochran S, Pillsbury S: Crisis Intervention Team Core Elements. Memphis, Tenn, University of Memphis School of Urban Affairs and Public Policy, Department of Criminology and Criminal Justice, CIT Center, 2007. Available at www.citinternational.org/images/stories/CIT/SectionImplementation/CoreElements.pdf. Accessed July 26, 2013Google Scholar

37 Improving Responses to People With Mental Illnesses: Tailoring Law Enforcement Initiatives to Individual Jurisdictions. New York, Council of State Governments Justice Center, 2010Google Scholar

38 Steadman HJ, Redlich A, Callahan L, et al.: Effect of mental health courts on arrests and jail days: a multisite study. Archives of General Psychiatry 68:167–172, 2011Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar