The American Psychiatric Association (APA) has updated its Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including with new information specifically addressed to individuals in the European Economic Area. As described in the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, this website utilizes cookies, including for the purpose of offering an optimal online experience and services tailored to your preferences.

Please read the entire Privacy Policy and Terms of Use. By closing this message, browsing this website, continuing the navigation, or otherwise continuing to use the APA's websites, you confirm that you understand and accept the terms of the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including the utilization of cookies.

×
Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ps.201300108

Abstract

Objective

The crisis intervention team (CIT) model is a widely implemented police-based program to improve officers’ responses to individuals with behavioral disorders. The authors examined levels of force used by officers with or without CIT training and disposition decisions in a large sample of encounters with individuals whom they suspected of having a serious mental illness, a drug or an alcohol problem, or a developmental disability.

Methods

A total of 180 officers (91 with CIT training and 89 without) in six departments reported on 1,063 encounters, including level of force and disposition (resolution at the scene, referral or transport to services, or arrest).

Results

CIT training status was generally not predictive of level of force, although CIT-trained officers were significantly more likely to report verbal engagement or negotiation as the highest level of force used (odds ratio [OR]=2.00, p=.016). For CIT-trained officers, referral or transport was a more likely outcome (OR=1.70, p=.026) and arrest was less likely (OR=.47, p=.007) than for officers without CIT training; these findings were most pronounced when physical force was necessary. Analyses of disposition differences by officers’ perceptions of subjects’ primary problem (for example, mental illness only versus a drug or an alcohol problem) found some effects of CIT training status.

Conclusions

CIT training appears to increase the likelihood of referral or transport to mental health services and decrease the likelihood of arrest during encounters with individuals thought to have a behavioral disorder. Research should address subject- and system-level outcomes that complement this early evidence of successful prebooking jail diversion.

Persons with serious mental illnesses are overrepresented in jails and prisons (1), and some of their charges likely stem from officers’ misinterpretation of disorder-related behaviors (2). The criminalization of serious mental illnesses may be partly a manifestation of insufficient training on the front lines (3,4). During patrol duties, law enforcement officers, who often take on a mental health triage role, encounter many persons with serious mental illnesses (as well as alcohol and drug problems and developmental disabilities). Most officers are unaware of the signs and symptoms of mental illnesses and available resources (or they lack confidence in the latter). Therefore, they might arrest subjects rather than refer them to psychiatric services, even when arrest is discretionary— perhaps viewing individuals as responsible for their illnesses or better off separated from the community (5). (For brevity and consistency with common policing terminology, we use “subject” to denote the individual with whom the officer interacts.) Recognition of criminalization has prompted the implementation of jail diversion programs nationwide (6). The crisis intervention team (CIT) model, based on the original Memphis program (7), has become increasingly popular among law enforcement agencies as a means of prebooking jail diversion.

The CIT model is a collaborative effort among the law enforcement, advocacy, and mental health communities, designed to improve outcomes of encounters between individuals with serious mental illnesses and the police. Key goals are improving the safety of the officer and the subject, minimizing use of force, and facilitating referral to treatment in lieu of incarceration, when appropriate. A fundamental aspect of the CIT model is a 40-hour training that provides officers with knowledge and techniques essential to identifying signs and symptoms of mental illnesses, deescalating crisis situations, and making appropriate dispositions. Because officers’ decision making is shaped by both formal and informal rules applied to address each encounter’s unique characteristics (8), CIT training may shift dispositional decisions when arrest is not obligatory.

Although enhanced knowledge and attitudes, reduced stigma, and improved self-efficacy and skills among CIT-trained officers have been reported (911), little is known about their use of force and dispositional decisions. In this study of officers’ encounters, we first examined the effects of CIT training on seven levels of force and three dispositions (resolution at the scene with no further action, referral to services or transport to a treatment facility, or arrest). We then assessed how level of force was related to disposition and whether that relation was affected by CIT training status. A third, exploratory set of analyses described how CIT training status affected disposition separately in four subsamples based on subjects’ suspected condition.

Methods

Participating law enforcement agencies and officers

This report is based on a sample of police encounters that occurred in Georgia from March to November 2010. To increase both the size and representativeness of the sample, we engaged six diverse, though primarily urban and suburban, law enforcement agencies (Table 1) that had implemented CIT within three to five years of the study and that had a relatively high percentage of CIT-trained officers among their sworn personnel (from 12% in Atlanta to 40% in Savannah).

Table 1 Characteristics of the six participating law enforcement agenciesa
CharacteristicAtlantaSavannah/ Chatham CountyClayton CountyCherokee CountybHenry CountyRome/Floyd County
N of sworn personnel1,78650835634426092
N of uniform patrol officers1,10016820512317563
N of CIT-trained officers210202119608630
N of CIT-trained supervisors15172816205
Month/year when CIT training began12/041/056/058/049/067/07

a This information was collected from an officer with crisis intervention team (CIT) training from each agency who served as a liaison to the study team.

b Cherokee County is a sheriff’s office; all others are police departments.

Table 1 Characteristics of the six participating law enforcement agenciesa
Enlarge table

We analyzed reports of 1,063 encounters provided by 180 officers (91 with CIT training and 89 without). The modal number of encounters reported per officer was four, and the number submitted did not vary significantly by CIT training. Initially, 586 officers who earlier had participated in a study of CIT training’s effects on officers’ knowledge, attitudes, and skills (12) were invited to participate; 397 (68%) enrolled. Written informed consent was obtained through a process approved by the Emory University Institutional Review Board, and these officers received a booklet containing 30 encounter forms and were asked to “complete a form for every interaction with a person who you think has a serious mental illness, alcohol or drug problem, or developmental disability” during a six-week period. A total of 180 officers (45%) returned a booklet with at least one completed encounter form. Data were reported for 1,063 encounters, although some data were missing for some encounters (see below).

Encounter form and encounter variables

The encounter form was developed after reviewing literature and forms used for reporting by various CIT programs. After initial development, the form was refined through two waves of review by police officers, CIT program coordinators, and others. The resulting form, a doubled-sided page consisting mostly of check-boxes, can be completed in one to two minutes.

The primary variables derived from the encounter form for this report were level of force and disposition. For level of force, officers checked which of several techniques or equipment was used (no level of force was checked for 28 encounters; N=1,035) We defined seven levels of force and coded each encounter by the highest that applied. Thus level 1 was coded if the officer checked only “My physical presence was enough” (N=404 encounters, 39%) and level 7 if the officer checked “I pushed, hit, grabbed, or otherwise physically engaged the subject” or used physical maneuvers (for example, soft or hard empty hands) or devices to handle the situation (N=121, 12%).

For disposition, encounters were coded as resolution at the scene with no further action (N=505, 48%), referral to services or transport to a treatment facility (N=361, 34%), or arrest (N=197, 19%). Disposition was recorded for all encounters (N=1,063).

Officers also recorded whether the subject was suspected to have a serious mental illness, a drug or an alcohol problem, or a developmental disability (they could check more than one). We defined four subsamples: mental illness only (N=422, 40%; including 24 encounters in which developmental disability was also checked), mental illness plus a drug or an alcohol problem (N=104, 10%; including eight encounters in which developmental disability was also checked), a drug or an alcohol problem only (N=362, 34%; including eight encounters in which developmental disability also checked), and disability or nothing indicated (N=175, 16%; including 82 encounters in which only developmental disability was checked and 93 encounters in which nothing was checked). We pooled the final two groups because analyses suggested that they did not differ meaningfully with respect to our primary variables.

Statistical analyses

We used SPSS, version 18, to recode and examine distributional properties of variables. Descriptive statistics are reported primarily in percentages. Odds ratios (ORs) were used to characterize effects of officers’ CIT training status. Because encounters were nested within officers, ORs were estimated with MPlus, version 6.1, which accommodates multilevel models, including those with binary outcomes. These ORs are >1 when the CIT percentage is higher than the non-CIT percentage and <1 when it is not. Generally, ORs with 95% confidence intervals (CIs) excluding 1 (no effect) are interpreted as significant. However, CIs of quite small or quite large ORs may fail to exclude 1 when cell numbers are small (that is, low power). In such cases, a useful guideline is that OR values ≤.33 or ≥3.00 indicate strong effects, >.33 but ≤.50 or ≥2.00 but <3.00 indicate medium effects, and >.50 but ≤.80 or ≥1.25 but <2.00 indicate weak effects (13).

Results

Descriptive characteristics

The mean±SD age and years of service of the 180 officers who submitted encounter forms were 36.6±8.0 and 9.7±7.8, respectively. A total of 135 (75%) were men; 93 (52%) were Caucasian, 75 (42%) African American, eight (4%) Hispanic, two (1%) Native American or Pacific Islander, and two (1%) Asian. We compared the 180 officers in this study with the 406 officers who participated in the earlier study (12). The officers in this study were more likely to be women (N=45, 25%, versus N=69, 17%, p=.024) and to have CIT training (N=92, 51%, versus N=162, 40%, p=.014); however, the two samples did not differ significantly with respect to age, race-ethnicity, or years of service. Of the 1,029 subjects involved in the encounters where gender was recorded, 628 (59%) were male; of the 1,052 whose age was recorded, 535 (50%) were at least 40 years old; and of the 1,050 whose race-ethnicity was recorded, 652 (62%) were African American, 349 (33%) were Caucasian, 40 (4%) were Hispanic, four (<1%) were Asian, and five (<1%) were mixed-race or not specified.

Effects of CIT training on level of force and disposition

Figure 1 shows the number of encounters coded for each level of force and disposition by officers with and without CIT training. Figure 2 shows the percentage of encounters coded for each level of force and disposition by officers with and without CIT training. The second most frequently used level of force was “verbally engaged or negotiated with the subject” (N=158 encounters, 15%). This was the only level of force that showed a statistically significant effect of CIT training status (OR=2.00, p=.016). This was the highest level of force used by CIT-trained officers in 20% of their encounters (N=100 of 503 encounters); however, for officers without CIT training, this was the highest level of force used in only 11% of their encounters (N=58 of 532 encounters (Figure 2).

Figure 1 Number of encounters by level of force and disposition reported by officers with or without crisis intervention team (CIT) traininga

a Officers coded 1,035 encounters by level of force and 1,063 by disposition. Percentages may not sum to 100 because of rounding.

Figure 2 Percentage of encounters reported by officers with or without crisis intervention team (CIT) training by level of force and dispositiona

a CIT-trained officers coded 503 encounters by level of force and 517 by disposition. Officers without CIT training coded 532 encounters by level of force and 546 by disposition. Percentages within level of force and disposition may not sum to 100 because of rounding. Odds ratios (ORs) indicate the magnitude of the difference (reference group: officers without CIT training).

b The 95% confidence interval does not include 1.

In regard to disposition, nearly half of encounters were resolved at the scene; the percentages were similar for officers with or without CIT training (Figures 1 and 2). In contrast, referral to services or transport to a treatment facility was more likely for CIT-trained officers than for officers without CIT training (40% versus 29%; OR=1.70, p=.026) and arrest was less likely (13% versus 24%; OR=.47, p=.007) (Figure 2).

Effects of level of force on disposition

For conceptual clarity in these analyses and to avoid problems with small sample sizes, we collapsed the seven levels into two levels: no or low force (levels 1–5, N=812, 78%) and physical force (levels 6–7, N=223, 22%). Figure 3 shows the percentage of encounters coded by the two officer groups for no or low force and physical force, separately for encounters that resulted in resolution, referral or transport, or arrest. As expected, resolution at the scene was more likely when officers used no force or a low level of force, and arrest was more likely when officers used physical force. But CIT training also mattered: CIT-trained officers were more likely than officers without CIT training to refer or transport (OR=3.38) and less likely to arrest (OR=.28) when physical force was required. Moreover, encounters involving CIT-trained officers were more likely to be resolved at the scene when physical force was used (OR=1.90; although the p value indicates nonsignificance, the OR value indicates a weak effect).

Figure 3 Percentage of encounters reported by officers with or without crisis intervention team (CIT) training by level of force for encounters resulting in resolution, referral or transport, and arresta

a For CIT-trained officers, 406 encounters involved no or low force and 97 involved physical force. For officers without CIT training, again 406 encounters involved no or low force and 126 involved physical force. Odds ratios (ORs) indicate the magnitude of the difference (reference group: officers without CIT training).

b The 95% confidence interval does not include 1.

Variation in encounter outcomes by subjects’ suspected condition

As noted above, subjects were categorized into four subgroups: mental illness only, mental illness plus a drug or an alcohol problem, a drug or an alcohol problem only, and disability or nothing indicated. Figure 4 summarizes data on encounter dispositions for the four subgroups by officers with CIT training (N=517 encounters) and officers without CIT training (N=546 encounters). CIT-trained officers coded a larger proportion of encounters as mental illness only, compared with officers without CIT training (N=231, 45%, versus N=191, 35%). CIT-trained officers also coded a smaller proportion of encounters as a drug or an alcohol problem only (N=132, 26%, versus N=230, 42%).

Figure 4 Dispositions of encounters reported by officers with or without crisis intervention team (CIT) training, by officers’ perception of subjects' conditiona

a Numbers to the left of each pair of bars are odds ratios indicating the effect of CIT training status for the adjacent disposition (reference group: officers without CIT training).

Figure 4 also provides ORs (reference group: officers without CIT training) for each encounter disposition by subgroup of subjects. The ORs are more informative than p values in this analysis because of the relatively small subsamples, which led to reduced power. As shown in Figure 2, when all encounters were considered, CIT training status had no effect on resolution at the scene. However, when the analyses examined resolution at the scene by subject subgroup, the OR values indicated no effect of CIT training for those with mental illness only, weak effects for those with mental illness plus a drug or an alcohol problem and for those with a drug or an alcohol problem only (resolution at the scene being less likely among CIT-trained officers), and a weak effect for the disability or nothing indicated group (more likely among CIT-trained officers) (Figure 4). The OR value for referral or transport for all encounters showed a weak effect (more likely among CIT-trained officers); for subject subgroups, effects were likewise weak for those with mental illness only and those with mental illness plus a drug or an alcohol problem, but effects were medium for those with a drug or an alcohol problem only. Finally, the OR value for arrest for all encounters showed a medium effect (less likely among CIT-trained officers); for subject subgroups, OR values indicated a medium effect for those with mental illness only, a weak effect for those with a drug or an alcohol problem only, and a strong effect for the disability or nothing indicated group.

Discussion

Several new findings emerged from this study. With regard to level of force, it is noteworthy that only 12% of encounters escalated to the level of physical maneuvers or force and that there was no difference in use of force between officers with CIT training and those without it. A probable explanation for this finding is that use of physical force is driven primarily by the violence potential of the encounter and the subject’s level of resistance, as has been shown in prior research (14), as well as by officers’ standard training and by policies on the force continuum, in which the officer’s response is driven by the subject’s resistance (15). Morabito and colleagues (16) reported that among 216 officers in four police districts in Chicago, subjects’ physical resistance was the strongest predictor of force (OR=20.15). However, an interaction was found between the officer’s CIT status and the subject’s resistant demeanor, such that CIT-trained officers were less likely to use force than officers without CIT training as the subject’s demeanor became more resistant.

Although use of physical force in our study did not differ between officers with or without CIT training, one of the seven levels did differ. Specifically, for 20% of encounters involving CIT-trained officers, the highest level of force used was verbal engagement or negotiation with the subject, whereas this was the highest level of force used in only 11% of encounters involving officers without CIT training. This difference suggests that the 40-hour CIT training, which focuses heavily on experiential and role-play training in verbal deescalation, enhances officers’ use of these techniques in encounters. A previous survey-based study (17), in which participants responded to a vignette describing an escalating scenario involving a man with psychosis, demonstrated that compared with officers who had no CIT training, CIT officers opted to use less force and perceived physical force to be less effective and nonphysical actions to be more effective. The findings of that study and other CIT studies, along with the findings reported here, indicate that the CIT model has an impact on officers’ actions and use-of-force decisions, although in complex ways.

Regarding encounter dispositions, referral to services or transport to a treatment facility was significantly more likely and arrest was significantly less likely when encounters involved CIT-trained officers. Steadman and colleagues (18) initially reported a lower arrest rate for a CIT program compared with other models (community service officers and a mobile crisis unit). Teller and coworkers (19) documented an increased rate of transport of persons in mental health crisis by CIT-trained officers. Watson and colleagues (20) found that compared with their peers with no CIT training, CIT-trained officers in Chicago resolved a greater proportion of calls involving persons with mental illnesses by linking them to mental health services; however, they found no difference in arrest rates. In the CIT model, a core element is a designated emergency mental health drop-off site (or multiple sites) with a no-refusal policy, which is key to improving officers’ willingness and ability to access services (21,22). Thus the 40-hour training likely works in conjunction with other elements of the CIT model to bring about desired outcomes, such as increased referral and diversion from arrest and incarceration to mental health services.

It is interesting that the effects of CIT on referral and arrest were strong and statistically significant when physical force was required and weak and not statistically significant when physical force was not used. Thus, when CIT-trained officers must revert to using force (which, as noted above, is driven primarily by violence potential of the encounter and the subject’s level of resistance), they are nonetheless more likely than officers without CIT training to refer the subject to services and less likely to execute an arrest. The likelihood of referral or arrest differed little between the two groups in encounters in which force was not required.

The sample of encounters was divided into four subsamples on the basis of the type of behavioral condition the officer suspected. Although subdividing the sample limited statistical power, the magnitude of several ORs suggested that the effects of CIT training status on dispositional decisions may vary by the officer’s perception of the subject’s primary problem. Encounters involving CIT-trained officers were less likely to result in arrest, especially for subjects who were suspected of having a mental illness only. On the other hand, encounters involving CIT-trained officers were most likely to result in referral or transport to mental health services when subjects were suspected of having a drug or an alcohol problem only.

We acknowledge five methodological limitations. First, because of the limited number of encounters reported by officers in specific agencies, it was not feasible to include the agency as an analytic variable. However, our intent was to study encounters, not departmental differences. Given the state-level coordination of CIT across jurisdictions in Georgia (23) and the fact that CIT was well established in each agency, we had no hypotheses concerning departmental variables. Second, although 397 officers provided informed consent and received encounter forms, only 180 returned completed forms. We do not know whether the remaining officers later decided not to participate, failed to document any encounters, did not have any pertinent encounters during the six weeks, or recorded data on encounter forms but did not submit them. Officers’ participation may have been associated with factors that created a selection bias; for example, CIT-trained officers may have been more likely to complete encounter forms when they thought they did a particularly good job. However, the encounter form intentionally did not include officers’ CIT status; they were categorized as CIT trained or not CIT trained using data from the prior survey (12).

Third, officers without CIT training documented 191 encounters in which they suspected that the subject had a serious mental illness; this figure was 231 for officers with CIT training (21% higher). However, in the CIT model, trained officers can be dispatched to calls in which the subject is believed to have a mental illness. Therefore, one might expect that compared with officers with no CIT training, CIT-trained officers would have reported many more encounters with individuals with mental illnesses. However, dispatch is often unaware that a mental illness is a factor in a call; officers commonly determine that a subject has a mental illness only when they arrive on the scene. Fourth, because officers could have been selective in completing encounter forms, we cannot assume that the arrest and referral rates observed in this large sample of encounters are equal to the arrest and referral rates for all encounters. These encounters were a sample; other methods would be required to determine arrest and referral rates for all encounters, which could then be compared with those of other jurisdictions. Finally, interpretation of most research on CIT is complicated by the fact that officers who undertake the training may differ in personal attributes (for example, a personal or family history of mental health treatment) from those who do not, which might confound differences between groups. Officers typically volunteer for CIT training, and are likely to be motivated to engage more productively with people with behavioral disorders. Additional research should address potential subject-level outcomes while attempting to control for such factors.

Conclusions

Although CIT training did not have a prominent effect on use of force, our finding that CIT-trained officers were more likely than officers without CIT training to refer subjects to services or transport them to a treatment facility, and less likely to execute an arrest, suggests the effectiveness of CIT as a form of prebooking jail diversion. Research is needed on system-level outcomes (for example, criminal justice cost savings and better collaboration between the criminal justice and mental health systems) that accompany the officer-level effects found in a related study (12) and the promising evidence reported here of effectiveness in prebooking jail diversion.

Dr. Compton and Ms. Broussard are with the Department of Psychiatry, Lenox Hill Hospital, the North Shore–LIJ Health System, New York City (e-mail: ). When this study was conducted, they were with Emory University, Atlanta, and The George Washington University, Washington, D.C. Dr. Bakeman is with the Department of Psychology, Georgia State University, Atlanta. Ms. Hankerson-Dyson, Ms. Husbands, Ms. Krishan, Ms. Stewart-Hutto, and Dr. D’Orio are with the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Emory University School of Medicine, Atlanta. Dr. Oliva was formerly with the Georgia Bureau of Investigation, Atlanta, and is now retired. Dr. Thompson is with the Department of Behavioral Sciences and Health Education, Rollins School of Public Health, Emory University, Atlanta. Dr. Watson is with the Jane Addams College of Social Work, University of Illinois at Chicago.

Acknowledgments and disclosures

The research reported in this article was supported by the National Institute of Mental Health under award number R01 MH082813 to Dr. Compton. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health.

The authors report no competing interests.

References

1 Ditton PM: Mental Health and Treatment of Inmates and Probationers: Special Report. Washington, DC, US Department of Justice, Bureau of Justice Statistics, 1999Google Scholar

2 Lamb HR, Weinberger LE, DeCuir WJ: The police and mental health. Psychiatric Services 53:1266–1271, 2002LinkGoogle Scholar

3 Torrey EF, Steiber J, Ezekiel J, et al.: Criminalizing the Seriously Mentally Ill: The Abuse of Jails as Mental Hospitals. Washington, DC, Public Citizen’s Health Research Group and National Alliance for the Mentally Ill, 1992Google Scholar

4 Watson AC, Morabito MS, Draine J, et al.: Improving police response to persons with mental illness: a multi-level conceptualization of CIT. International Journal of Law and Psychiatry 31:359–368, 2008Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

5 Watson AC, Ottati V, Lurigio A, et al.: Police and stigma; in A Comprehensive Review of the Stigma of Mental Illness: Implications for Research and Social Change. Edited by Corrigan PW. Washington, DC, American Psychological Association, 2005CrossrefGoogle Scholar

6 Munetz MR, Griffin PA: Use of the sequential intercept model as an approach to decriminalization of people with serious mental illness. Psychiatric Services 57:544–549, 2006LinkGoogle Scholar

7 Dupont R, Cochran S: Police response to mental health emergencies—barriers to change. Journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law 28:338–344, 2000MedlineGoogle Scholar

8 Wood J, Swanson J, Burris S, et al: Police Interventions With Persons Affected by Mental Illnesses: A Critical Review of Global Thinking and Practice. New Brunswick, NJ, Rutgers University, Center for Behavioral Health Services and Criminal Justice Research, March 2011. Available at papers.ssrn.com/sol3/papers.cfm?abstract_id=1781909Google Scholar

9 Borum R, Deane MW, Steadman HJ, et al.: Police perspectives on responding to mentally ill people in crisis: perceptions of program effectiveness. Behavioral Sciences and the Law 16:393–405, 1998Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

10 Compton MT, Esterberg ML, McGee R, et al.: Crisis intervention team training: changes in knowledge, attitudes, and stigma related to schizophrenia. Psychiatric Services 57:1199–1202, 2006LinkGoogle Scholar

11 Bahora M, Hanafi S, Chien VH, et al.: Preliminary evidence of effects of crisis intervention team training on self-efficacy and social distance. Administration and Policy in Mental Health and Mental Health Services Research 35:159–167, 2008Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

12 Compton MT, Bakeman R, Broussard B, et al.: The police-based crisis intervention team (CIT) model: I. effects on officers’ knowledge, attitudes, and skills. Psychiatric Services, 2013; doi 10.1176/appi.ps.201300107Google Scholar

13 Bakeman R, Quera V: Sequential Analysis and Observational Methods for the Behavioral Sciences. Cambridge, United Kingdom, Cambridge University Press, 2011CrossrefGoogle Scholar

14 Riksheim EC, Chermak SM: Causes of police behavior revisited. Journal of Criminal Justice 21:353–382, 1993CrossrefGoogle Scholar

15 Alpert GP, Dunham RG: The Force Factor: Measuring and Assessing Police Use of Force and Suspect Resistance. Washington, DC, US Department of Justice, 1999Google Scholar

16 Morabito MS, Kerr AN, Watson A, et al.: Crisis Intervention Teams and people with mental illness: exploring the factors that influence use of force. Crime and Delinquency 58:57–77, 2012CrossrefGoogle Scholar

17 Compton MT, Demir Neubert BN, Broussard B, et al.: Use of force preferences and perceived effectiveness of actions among Crisis Intervention Team (CIT) police officers and non-CIT officers in an escalating psychiatric crisis involving a subject with schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin 37:737–745, 2011Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

18 Steadman HJ, Deane MW, Borum R, et al.: Comparing outcomes of major models of police responses to mental health emergencies. Psychiatric Services 51:645–649, 2000LinkGoogle Scholar

19 Teller JLS, Munetz MR, Gil KM, et al.: Crisis intervention team training for police officers responding to mental disturbance calls. Psychiatric Services 57:232–237, 2006LinkGoogle Scholar

20 Watson AC, Ottati VC, Morabito M, et al.: Outcomes of police contacts with persons with mental illness: the impact of CIT. Administration and Policy in Mental Health and Mental Health Services Research 37:302–317, 2010Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

21 Dupont R, Cochran S, Pillsbury S: Crisis Intervention Team Core Elements. Memphis, Tenn, University of Memphis, School of Urban Affairs and Public Policy, Department of Criminology and Criminal Justice, CIT Center, 2007. Available at www.citinternational.org/images/stories/CIT/SectionImplementation/CoreElements.pdf. Accessed July 26, 2013Google Scholar

22 Compton MT, Broussard B, Hankerson-Dyson D, et al.: System- and policy-level challenges to full implementation of the Crisis Intervention Team (CIT) model. Journal of Police Crisis Negotiations 10:72–85, 2010Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

23 Oliva JR, Compton MT: A statewide Crisis Intervention Team (CIT) initiative: evolution of the Georgia CIT program. Journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law 36:38–46, 2008MedlineGoogle Scholar