The American Psychiatric Association (APA) has updated its Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including with new information specifically addressed to individuals in the European Economic Area. As described in the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, this website utilizes cookies, including for the purpose of offering an optimal online experience and services tailored to your preferences.

Please read the entire Privacy Policy and Terms of Use. By closing this message, browsing this website, continuing the navigation, or otherwise continuing to use the APA's websites, you confirm that you understand and accept the terms of the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including the utilization of cookies.

×

Abstract

OBJECTIVE: Until recently, few training methods used in the psychosocial rehabilitation of persons with schizophrenia have taken into account the neurocognitive deficits common to this illness. The present study tested a training method called errorless learning (which theoretically compensates for neurocognitive impairments) for efficacy at teaching entry-level job tasks to persons with serious and persistent mental illness. METHOD: Sixty-five unemployed, clinically stable outpatients with schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder were randomly assigned to training by means of either errorless learning or conventional instruction for two entry-level job tasks (index card filing and toilet tank assembly). Training was conducted in small groups of four subjects for 90–120 minutes in a simulated workshop. Efficacy—as measured by accuracy, speed, and overall productivity—was assessed immediately after training and at a 3-month follow-up evaluation. RESULTS: Significant group differences in accuracy that favored errorless learning were found on both job tasks. In terms of productivity, the errorless learning group was superior to the conventional instruction group on the card filing task, and differences for the tank assembly task approached significance. There were no significant group differences in speed. Both groups showed significant drops in accuracy and productivity on the tank assembly task from immediately after training to the 3-month follow-up evaluation; performance levels were more stable for both groups on the card filing task. CONCLUSIONS: Errorless learning appears to be a useful training method for establishing introductory high levels of performance in the work rehabilitation of persons with schizophrenia. Future studies may wish to test its long-term durability by embedding it within supported employment programs.

Poor tenure, productivity, and satisfaction with work are major problems for most persons with schizophrenia. Historically, estimates of the rates of competitive employment among individuals with schizophrenia have been markedly low, ranging from 10% to 20% (1). More recently, supported employment (2) has been successful in helping over 50% of those with severe mental illness, and approximately 40% with schizophrenia, to obtain competitive employment, usually on a part-time basis (35). However, among those receiving supported employment services, only half maintain their jobs for more than 6 months (6, 7).

The reasons for job termination among persons with severe mental illness include high levels of negative symptoms, poor social skills, interpersonal stressors, lack of family support, relapses consequent to medication noncompliance, dissatisfaction with work, and poor work quality (810). Interventions that can remedy some of these obstacles to sustained employment are available (1113), but few treatment initiatives have aimed at facilitating the learning, mastery, and sustained performance of job tasks subsequent to employment. Improved techniques for teaching job tasks may be needed to take into account the neurocognitive deficits of schizophrenia that can impede the acquisition and performance of functional skills (14, 15).

A number of different treatments have been recently proposed and tested for their efficacy at ameliorating neurocognitive deficits in schizophrenia (1623), most of which fall under the broader categories of restorative versus compensatory approaches (24). Restorative efforts attempt to improve neurocognitive function by targeting the deficit directly through either pharmacological or psychosocial interventions. In contrast to restorative approaches, compensatory ones attempt to improve function by recruiting relatively intact neurocognitive processes to fill the role of impaired ones or by using prosthetic aids to compensate for the loss of function (25).

In the present study, a compensatory training method, based on principles of errorless learning (26, 27), was designed and tested for its efficacy at teaching persons with schizophrenia entry-level job tasks. Errorless learning minimizes the demands on explicit memory, which is impaired in schizophrenia (28, 29), and instead relies more on implicit memory, which some but not all studies report to be relatively intact (3033). This approach assumes that new learning is stronger and more durable if mistakes are eliminated during training. Performance becomes automated through imitative learning and repetitive practice of perfect task execution. Errorless learning has been used extensively in the clinical care and management of children with neurodevelopmental disabilities (3436) and, more recently, in the rehabilitation of amnestic and schizophrenic patients (3740).

Although healthy individuals can recruit neurocognitive resources needed to correct past mistakes and guide future behavior, many persons with schizophrenia cannot (41, 42). By reducing the task to its basic components, each associated with a high likelihood for success, errorless learning minimizes failure during the acquisition of new skills and reduces the need for self-correction. This approach stands in marked contrast to the type of instruction typically provided to persons when they begin a new job. In such situations, new employees are commonly given verbal instruction, a demonstration, practice time, and then feedback about their performance. Training in this way generally leads to the commission of several mistakes that can be hard to correct because of difficulties in learning from past mistakes.

The present study aimed to test the hypothesis that errorless learning would be more effective than conventional trial-and-error training for teaching persons with schizophrenia entry-level job tasks as well as determine the extent to which the benefits of errorless learning lasted at a 3-month follow-up evaluation.

Method

Subjects

Sixty-five subjects who met DSM-IV criteria for schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder from the Department of Veterans Affairs Greater Los Angeles Healthcare Center and the San Fernando Mental Health Center participated in the study. Psychiatric diagnosis was determined after administration of the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders Patient Edition (SCID-I/P) (43) by an interviewer trained to use the SCID-I/P by the Diagnosis and Psychopathology Unit of the University of California, Los Angeles, Clinical Research Center for the Study of Schizophrenia. A minimum kappa of 0.75 for rating the presence of psychotic and mood items is required of all interviewers trained in the program. Subjects were clinically stable outpatients who had no psychiatric hospitalizations in the past 6 months, had been maintained on the same antipsychotic medication for the past 3 months, and had been unemployed for the past year. Antipsychotic medication type and dose were not controlled in the study but were left to the discretion of the subject’s treating physician. An abbreviated measure of overall work capacity (44, 45) was used to screen potential participants who were good performers (i.e., greater than 90% accuracy) to reduce ceiling effects as well as provide a rough indication of baseline performance. Subjects also completed a neurocognitive assessment before training to explore whether errorless learning compensates for specific cognitive deficits. These data are available elsewhere (46). Table 1 presents the demographic, chronicity, and symptom characteristics for the study group, divided by method of training. After complete description of the study to the subjects, written informed consent was obtained.

Procedure

Following random assignment to training group (errorless learning or conventional instruction), all subjects received training on two entry-level job tasks: index card filing and toilet tank assembly. The tasks have standardized scoring procedures and good social and construct validity (44, 45). The order of training on the job tasks was counterbalanced across groups. Training was conducted in small groups, usually with four subjects (range=2–4) in a simulated work environment with a supervisor and two assistants. Training was completed in a single session and lasted 45–60 minutes, depending on speed of skill acquisition. Separate workstations with dividers were set up so that subjects could not view the performance of others working on the same job task.

The groups were yoked for training time, in that the length of training for the errorless learning group dictated the length of training for the succeeding conventional instruction group. Training for the conventional instruction group included verbal instruction, a demonstration, time for independent practice, and supervisor feedback at the midpoint and end of the training session.

Training for the errorless learning group began with simpler task components and then proceeded stepwise to more difficult ones in which the task demands were progressively more complicated. At each stage, subjects met criteria for mastery by successfully completing a series of performance-based tasks without error. Training for the conventional instruction group involved more practice time and less didactic instruction compared with the errorless learning group. Training efficacy was assessed in separate 1-hour work sessions immediately after training particular to that job task and at a 3-month follow-up evaluation. Forty-eight of the 65 subjects were available for retest at the 3-month follow-up evaluation. Psychiatric symptoms were assessed within a 2-week window of training by using the 24-item Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale (BPRS) (47) and were administered by an interviewer trained to a minimum intraclass correlation coefficient of 0.80 by the Diagnosis and Psychopathology Unit.

Index Card Filing

This task involved filing 40-card piles of index cards. Subjects were told that each card contained information about a person who had purchased a car. Each card contained the city of purchase, car manufacturer, and owner’s last name printed in large (20-point) boldface type (Arial font). Subjects filed the cards into filing boxes according to city of purchase, car manufacturer, the alphabetical section for the owner’s last name (e.g., A-F, G-L), and alphabetical order according to the owner’s last name. Dependent variables included measures of accuracy ([total number of cards filed correctly/total number of cards filed] × 100), speed (total number of cards filed per unit time), and overall productivity (total number of cards filed correctly minus total number of cards misfiled).

Toilet Tank Assembly

This task involved assembling the kind of toilet tank commonly found in community home refurbishing stores. The 31-piece assembly kit included a flush valve, fill valve, and lever arm assemblies. Subjects were instructed to complete assembly of the entire kit. Analogous to card filing, the dependent variables for tank assembly included measures of accuracy ([total number of parts correctly assembled/total number of parts completed] × 100), speed (total number of tanks completed per unit time), and overall productivity (total number of parts assembled correctly minus total number of parts missing or incorrectly assembled).

On-Task Performance

Assessment of on-task behavior followed established time-sampling procedures (48) in which a timer signaled raters to conduct a 2-second observational sweep of subjects at 5-minute intervals throughout the duration of the two posttraining work sessions (immediately after training and at the 3-month follow-up evaluation). On-task performance was defined as the execution of a behavior required to complete the task (e.g., picking up an index card, looking on the table for a tank assembly piece). Examples of off-task behaviors included staring out the window or talking to another subject. On-task performance was scored as a dichotomous variable (0=off-task; 1=on-task). Assessment of interrater reliability yielded a kappa of 0.89.

Subject Satisfaction With Training

A 10-point Likert scale was administered to a subset of subjects (N=36) to measure the potential adverse effects of trainer bias and subject satisfaction with training (1=extremely negative, 10=extremely positive). Separate ratings were obtained for each job task.

Data Analyses

Initially, contrasts were conducted on demographic, chronicity, and symptom measures to examine possible group differences at the time of training. Correlational analyses were performed to examine possible significant relationships between symptoms and job task performance for the two training groups. The three dependent measures from each job task were then analyzed separately by using a group (errorless learning versus conventional instruction) by time (immediate versus 3-month follow-up) 2×2 repeated-measures analysis of covariance with baseline performance on the screening measure of work capacity entered as the lone covariate. Because of the skewed score distributions for accuracy on both job tasks, these data were log transformed. The card filing data were normalized after transformation and were analyzed by using the SAS mixed model procedure (49). The data from the tank assembly task could not be normalized and were thus analyzed by using the SAS general model procedure for Poisson distributions. Follow-up between- and within-group contrasts were conducted on all significant main (i.e., group, time) and interaction (i.e., group-by-time) effects. Between-group contrasts were also conducted on measures of on-task performance and subject satisfaction with training.

Results

There were no differences between the two groups on any demographic or chronicity measures, and the two groups were also comparable in their severity of symptoms at the time of training (Table 1). No significant correlations were found between symptoms (BPRS total, positive symptom, and negative symptom scores) and job task performance (accuracy, speed, and productivity) for either training group (conventional instruction: all p>0.18; errorless learning: all p>0.15).

Efficacy

Accuracy

For card filing, the results revealed a significant effect of group (F=4.92, df=1, 59, p=0.03), a nonsignificant effect of time (F=2.84, df=1, 44, p<0.10), and no interaction effects. That is, the errorless learning group showed better overall performance than the conventional instruction group, and performance in both groups declined over the 3-month follow-up period at about the same rate (Figure 1). Similar results were found for toilet tank assembly, for which there was a significant effect of group (χ2=4.39, df=1, p<0.04) and time (χ2=34.42, df=1, p<0.0001) but no group-by-time interaction. The errorless learning group showed better overall performance, but the performance of both groups declined significantly over the 3-month follow-up period (Figure 2).

Speed

For the card filing and tank assembly tasks, the results failed to reveal any significant group, time, or interaction effects (all p>0.15).

Productivity

For card filing, there was a significant effect of group (F=7.18, df=1, 59, p<0.01) but no significant time or interaction effects. The errorless learning group showed better overall productivity than the conventional instruction group, and the level of productivity of the groups was relatively stable over time. For tank assembly, there was a nonsignificant training effect differential that favored the errorless learning group (F=3.30, df=1, 60, p<0.08), and there was a significant effect of time (F=8.64, df=1, 44, p<0.006), but there was no group-by-time interaction. The overall performance of the errorless learning group tended to be better than the conventional instruction group, but both groups showed a decline in productivity over time.

On-Task Performance

The two groups were highly comparable in their on-task performance during the assessment periods conducted immediately after training (card filing: t=0.379, df=61, p=0.71; tank assembly: t=–1.105, df=62, p=0.27). The percentage of time on task was quite high during card filing (conventional instruction: mean=99.1% [SD=3.5%]; errorless learning: mean=99.1% [SD=2.9%]) and tank assembly (conventional instruction: mean=98.6% [SD=6.5%]; errorless learning: mean=99.1% [SD=3.6%]). Similar results were obtained for the measurements obtained at the 3-month follow-up evaluation.

Subject Satisfaction With Training

The results revealed the two groups to be highly comparable in their ratings of the training procedures for both job tasks (card filing: t=0.29, df=34, p=0.78; tank assembly: t=–0.07, df=34, p=0.95). In addition, the ratings were high for both groups across job tasks, indicating that the vast majority of subjects, regardless of training group, favorably experienced the training procedures for card filing (conventional instruction: mean=8.84 [SD=1.54]; errorless learning: mean=8.71 [SD=1.26]) and tank assembly (conventional instruction: mean=8.79 [SD=1.62]; errorless learning: mean=8.82 [SD=1.38]).

Discussion

The present study examined the efficacy of errorless learning at teaching two entry-level job tasks to a group of unemployed, clinically stable outpatients with schizophrenia and schizoaffective disorder. The results showed that errorless learning was better than conventional instruction at improving accuracy but had little effect on speed. Of note, the level of accuracy on the card filing task after errorless training was roughly comparable to the level attained by a group of healthy comparison subjects who took part in an earlier project that used this same measure (45) (healthy subjects: mean=95.0%, SD=8.1%; patients: mean=91.8%, SD=11.4%). On a measure of productivity that took into consideration speed and accuracy, the errorless learning group performed better than the conventional instruction group on the card filing task, and there was a trend in the same direction for the tank assembly task.

Why might errorless learning be an effective training intervention for persons with schizophrenia? It has been suggested that among the neurocognitive deficits associated with schizophrenia, the disturbances to explicit memory and related systems are disproportionately large (28, 29). In contrast, some, but not all, studies report implicit or procedural-based learning to be relatively intact (3033). It stands to reason that a training intervention that minimizes the demands on impaired neurocognitive processes and instead emphasizes the recruitment of more intact ones might be a better training intervention with this population. From a more experiential perspective, one advantage of errorless learning is that subjects are largely insulated from the experience of failure and the potentially negative cascading effect this experience may have on skill acquisition.

Regardless of its theoretical underpinnings, the efficacy of errorless learning as examined in the present study should be interpreted vis-à-vis certain potentially key influences on performance outcome. Observed effects could be due to differences in the amount of time spent in training-related activities, trainer bias, on-task behavior, or baseline characteristics. As part of the study design, total time engaged in training-related activities was equated for the two groups. However, the amount of time devoted to certain aspects of training (e.g., instruction, practice) differed. For the errorless learning group, more time was allocated to trainer-provided instruction, whereas for the conventional instruction group more time was allocated to independent practice. Hence, the possibility that efficacy may be related to the amount of trainer-provided instruction cannot be ruled out. With respect to trainer bias, measured by participants’ satisfaction with the training, the results indicated that both groups perceived training quite favorably. Similarly, there were no group differences in on-task performance, and the overall level was quite high. Finally, the differential training effects do not appear to be due to differences in demographic, chronicity, or symptom characteristics, since the groups were roughly comparable on these variables, and symptoms also showed no significant relationship to job task performance.

When we examined the extent to which training effects were maintained over time, the results showed that performance accuracy dropped for both groups over time. Subject attrition and increased score variance may have contributed to the failure to detect differential maintenance of training effects. Consideration should also be given to the fact that subjects had no subsequent exposure to the job tasks from the time of training to the 3-month follow-up evaluation. This arrangement bears little relationship to the realities of the workplace, in which newly hired personnel are given ample opportunity to practice and consolidate their job skills. In addition, supported employment programs provide job coaches to be available indefinitely to reinforce job performance once tasks are learned. Thus, the experiment was an unusually stringent test of the durability of either training method.

The hypothesis that the errorless learning group would show a differential training effect on speed of performance was not supported. The training procedures for the present study were not designed to improve speed per se but rather accuracy. The prediction regarding speed was primarily based on observation of subject performance from an earlier study aimed at remediating performance deficits on the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (50) in which subjects’ sorting of cards appeared to be automated after training, hence requiring little cognitive effort in the decision-making process.

On the basis of these findings, and those from other studies (20, 37–40, 51, 52), there appears to be ample support for continued testing of errorless learning for use in work rehabilitation. Errorless learning can be applied to a wide variety of job tasks, particularly those typically provided to seriously mentally ill clients at entry-level positions (53). Future efforts may aim to test the effectiveness of errorless learning at actual work sites in the community with the teachers being job coaches. There are practical limitations to errorless learning. Obviously, the more diverse and multifaceted the job, the more difficult and time-consuming it would be to apply errorless learning training procedures. Even with relatively simple job tasks, errorless learning takes considerably longer than conventional methods of instruction. Studies examining the cost/benefit ratio of errorless learning are warranted.

TABLE 1

Presented in part at the 16th annual meeting of the Society for Research in Psychopathology, Madison, Wisc., Nov. 1–4, 2001. Received Dec. 7, 2001; revision received May 15, 2002; accepted May 22, 2002. From the UCLA Department of Psychiatry and Biobehavioral Sciences, Los Angeles; the VA Greater Los Angeles Healthcare System; the Department of Veterans Affairs VISN 22 Mental Illness Research Education and Clinical Center (MIRECC); and the San Fernando Mental Health Center, San Fernando, Calif. Address reprint requests to Dr. Kern, VA Greater Los Angeles Healthcare System (MIRECC 210 A), 11301 Wilshire Blvd., Los Angeles, CA 90073; (e-mail). Supported by the UCLA Clinical Research Center for the Study of Schizophrenia (R.P. Liberman, principal investigator), the Department of Veterans Affairs VISN 22 Mental Illness Research Education and Clinical Center, and NIMH grant MH-30911. This project received human subjects approval from the UCLA General Campus Institutional Review Board, the West Los Angeles VA Institutional Review Board, and the Los Angeles County Department of Mental Health’s Human Subjects Committee. The authors thank Sharon S. Mitchell, M.A., Debra Doran, Anita L. Herrera, M.S., and Katie Kogler for their collective efforts in the training and data collection; Sun Hwang, M.S., for the data analyses; Charles J. Wallace, Ph.D., for consultation; and the patients and staff of the San Fernando and VA Greater Los Angeles Healthcare Centers for their respective participation and support of this project.

Figure 1.

Figure 1. Card Filing Accuracy Among Outpatients With Schizophrenia or Schizoaffective Disorder Immediately After Being Trained Through Either Errorless Learning or Conventional Instruction and After 3 Monthsa

aSignificant group effect (F=4.92, df=1, 59, p=0.03).

Figure 2.

Figure 2. Toilet Tank Assembly Accuracy Among Outpatients With Schizophrenia or Schizoaffective Disorder Immediately After Being Trained Through Either Errorless Learning or Conventional Instruction and After 3 Monthsa

aSignificant effect of group (χ2=4.39, df=1, p<0.04) and time (χ2=34.42, df=1, p<0.0001).

References

1. Bond GR: Vocational rehabilitation, in Handbook of Psychiatric Rehabilitation. Edited by Liberman RP. Boston, Allyn & Bacon, 1992, pp 244-275Google Scholar

2. Becker DR, Drake RE: A Working Life: The Individual Placement and Support (IPS) Program. Concord, New Hampshire-Dartmouth Psychiatric Research Center, 1993Google Scholar

3. Cook JA, Razzano L: Vocational rehabilitation for persons with schizophrenia: recent research and implications for practice. Schizophr Bull 2000; 26:87-103Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

4. Drake RE, McHugo GJ, Bebout RR, Becker DR, Harris M, Bond GR, Quimby E: A randomized clinical trial of supported employment for inner-city patients with severe mental disorders. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1999; 56:627-633Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

5. Bond GR, Drake R, Mueser K, Becker D: An update on supported employment for people with severe mental illness. Psychiatr Serv 1997; 48:335-344LinkGoogle Scholar

6. McHugo GJ, Drake RE, Becker DR: The durability of supported employment effects. Psychiatr Rehabil J 1998; 22:55-61CrossrefGoogle Scholar

7. Xie H, Dain B, Becker DR, Drake RE: Job tenure among persons with severe mental illness. Rehabil Couns Bull 1997; 40:230-239Google Scholar

8. Becker DR, Drake RE, Bond GR, Xie H, Dain BJ, Harrison K: Job terminations among persons with severe mental illness participating in supported employment. Community Ment Health J 1998; 34:71-82Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

9. Hoffman H, Kupper Z: Relationships between social competence, psychopathology and work performance and their predictive value for vocational rehabilitation of schizophrenic outpatients. Schizophr Res 1997; 23:69-79Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

10. Jacobs HE, Wissusik D, Collier R, Stackman D, Burkeman D: Correlations between psychiatric disabilities and vocational outcome. Hosp Community Psychiatry 1992; 43:365-369AbstractGoogle Scholar

11. Mueser KT, Glynn SM: Behavioral Family Therapy for Psychiatric Disorders. Oakland, Calif, New Harbinger, 1999Google Scholar

12. Eckman TA, Wirshing WC, Marder SR, Liberman RP, Johnston-Cronk K, Zimmermann K, Mintz J: Technique for training schizophrenic patients in illness self-management: a controlled trial. Am J Psychiatry 1992; 149:1549-1555LinkGoogle Scholar

13. Liberman RP, Wallace CJ, Blackwell G, Eckman TA, Vaccaro JV, Kuehnel TG: Innovations in skills training for the seriously mentally ill: the UCLA Social & Independent Living Skills Modules. Innovations & Research 1993; 2:43-60Google Scholar

14. Green MF: What are the functional consequences of neurocognitive deficits in schizophrenia? Am J Psychiatry 1996; 153:321-330LinkGoogle Scholar

15. Green MF, Kern RS, Braff DL, Mintz J: Neurocognitive deficits and functional outcome in schizophrenia: are we measuring the “right stuff”? Schizophr Bull 2000; 26:119-136Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

16. Bell M, Gryson G, Greig T, Corcoran C, Wexler BE: Neurocognitive enhancement therapy with work therapy: effects on neuropsychological test performance. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2001; 58:763-768Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

17. Medalia A, Revheim H, Casey M: Remediation of memory disorders in schizophrenia. Psychol Med 2000; 30:1451-1459Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

18. Silverstein SM, Pierce DL, Saytes M, Hems L, Schenkel L, Streaker N: Behavioral treatment of attentional dysfunction in chronic, treatment-refractory schizophrenia. Psychiatr Q 1998; 69:95-105Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

19. Spaulding WD, Reed D, Sullivan M, Richardson C, Weiler M: Effects of cognitive treatment in psychiatric rehabilitation. Schizophr Bull 1999; 25:657-676Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

20. Wykes T, van der Gaag M: Is it time to develop a new cognitive therapy for psychosis—cognitive remediation therapy (CRT)? Clin Psychol Rev 2001; 21:1227-1256Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

21. Keefe RSE, Silva SG, Perkins DO, Lieberman JA: The effects of atypical antipsychotic drugs on neurocognitive impairment in schizophrenia: a review and meta-analysis. Schizophr Bull 1999; 25:201-222Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

22. Meltzer HY, McGurk SR: The effects of clozapine, risperidone, and olanzapine on cognitive function in schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull 1999; 25:233-256Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

23. Liberman RP: Cognitive remediation in schizophrenia, in Comprehensive Treatment of Schizophrenia: Linking Neurobehavioral Finding to Psychosocial Approaches. Edited by Kashima H, Mizuno M, Falloon IRH, Asai M. Tokyo, Springer-Verlag, 2001, pp 254-275Google Scholar

24. Park NW, Ingles JL: Effectiveness of attention training after an acquired brain injury: a meta-analysis of rehabilitation studies. Brain Cogn 2000; 44:5-9Google Scholar

25. Velligan DI, Bow-Thomas CC, Huntzinger C, Ritch J, Ledbetter N, Prihoda TJ, Miller AL: Randomized controlled trial of the use of compensatory strategies to enhance adaptive functioning in outpatients with schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 2000; 157:1317-1323LinkGoogle Scholar

26. Terrace HS: Errorless transfer of a discrimination across two continua. J Exp Anal Behav 1963; 6:223-232Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

27. Terrace HS: Stimulus control, in Operant Behavior: Areas of Research and Application. Edited by Honig WK. New York, Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1966, pp 271-344Google Scholar

28. Heinrichs RW, Zakzanis KK: Neurocognitive deficit in schizophrenia: a quantitative review of the evidence. Neuropsychology 1998; 12:426-445Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

29. Saykin AJ, Gur RC, Gur RE, Mozley PD, Mozley LH, Resnick SM, Kester DB, Stafiniak P: Neuropsychological function in schizophrenia: selective impairment in memory and learning. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1991; 48:618-624Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

30. Clare L, McKenna PJ, Mortimer AM, Baddeley AD: Memory in schizophrenia: what is impaired and what is spared? Neuropsychologia 1993; 31:1225-1241Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

31. Danion J-M, Meulemans T, Kauffmann-Muller F, Vermaat H: Intact implicit learning in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 2001; 158:944-948LinkGoogle Scholar

32. Kern RS, Green MF, Wallace CJ: Declarative and procedural learning in schizophrenia: a test of the integrity of divergent memory systems. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry 1997; 2:39-50CrossrefGoogle Scholar

33. Green MF, Kern RS: Procedural learning in schizophrenia: evidence from serial reaction time. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry 1997; 2:123-134CrossrefGoogle Scholar

34. Touchette PE, Howard JS: Errorless learning: reinforcement contingencies and stimulus control transfer in delayed prompting. J Appl Behav Anal 1984; 17:175-188Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

35. Lancioni GE, Smeets PM: Procedures and parameters of errorless discrimination training with developmentally impaired individuals. Int Rev Res Ment Retard 1986; 14:135-164CrossrefGoogle Scholar

36. Ducharme JM, Lucas H, Pontes E: Errorless embedding in the reduction of severe maladaptive behavior during interactive and learning tasks. Behav Ther 1994; 25:489-501CrossrefGoogle Scholar

37. Wilson BA: Rehabilitation and memory disorders, in Neuropsychology of Memory, 2nd ed. Edited by Squire LR, Butters N. New York, Guilford, 1992, pp 315-321Google Scholar

38. Baddeley AD: Implicit memory and errorless learning: a link between cognitive theory and neuropsychological rehabilitation? Ibid, pp 309-314Google Scholar

39. Kern RS, Wallace CJ, Hellman SG Womack L, Green MF: A training procedure for remediating WCST deficits in chronic psychotic patients: an adaption of errorless learning principles. J Psychiatr Res 1996; 30:283-294Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

40. O’Carroll RE, Russell HH, Lawrie SM, Johnstone EC: Errorless learning and the cognitive rehabilitation of memory-impaired schizophrenic patients. Psychol Med 1999; 29:105-112Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

41. Weinberger DR, Aloia MS, Goldberg TE, Berman KF: The frontal lobes and schizophrenia. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 1994; 6:419-427; correction, 1995; 7:121Google Scholar

42. Goldberg TE, Weinberger DR, Berman KF, Pliskin NH, Podd MH: Further evidence for dementia of the prefrontal type in schizophrenia? a controlled study of teaching the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1987; 44:1008-1014Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

43. First MB, Spitzer RL, Gibbon M, Williams JBW: Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders Patient Edition, version 2.0 (SCID-I/P). Washington, DC, American Psychiatric Press, 1996Google Scholar

44. Massel HK, Liberman RP, Mintz J, Jacobs HE, Rush T, Giannini C, Zarate R: Evaluating the capacity to work of the mentally ill. Psychiatry 1990; 53:31-44Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

45. Zarate R, Liberman RP, Mintz J, Massel HK: Validation of a work capacity evaluation in individuals with psychiatric disorders. J Rehabil 1998; 64:28-34Google Scholar

46. Kern RS, Green MF, Mintz J, Liberman RP: Does “errorless learning” compensate for neurocognitive impairments in the work rehabilitation of schizophrenic patients? Psychol Med (in press)Google Scholar

47. Lukoff D, Nuechterlein K, Ventura J: Manual for the Expanded Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale. Schizophr Bull 1986; 12:594-602Google Scholar

48. Paul GL: The Time-Sample Behavior Checklist. Champaign, Ill, Research Press, 1987Google Scholar

49. SAS Version 6.12. Cary, NC, SAS Institute, 1996Google Scholar

50. Heaton RK, Chelune GJ, Talley JL, Kay GG, Curtiss G: Wisconsin Card Sorting Test Manual. Odessa, Fla, Psychological Assessment Resources, 1993Google Scholar

51. Kern RS, Green MF: Cognitive remediation in schizophrenia, in Handbook of Social Functioning in Schizophrenia. Edited by Mueser KT, Tarrier N. Needham Heights, Mass, Allyn & Bacon, 1998, pp 342-354Google Scholar

52. Wykes T: Non-pharmacological rehabilitation of cognitive deficits, in Improving Cognitive Function in the Schizophrenic Patient. Edited by Keefe RSE. London, Science Press, 1999, pp 52-62Google Scholar

53. Baron RC, Salzer MS: The career patterns of persons with serious mental illness. Psychiatr Rehabilitation Skills 2000; 4:136-158CrossrefGoogle Scholar