The American Psychiatric Association (APA) has updated its Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including with new information specifically addressed to individuals in the European Economic Area. As described in the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, this website utilizes cookies, including for the purpose of offering an optimal online experience and services tailored to your preferences.

Please read the entire Privacy Policy and Terms of Use. By closing this message, browsing this website, continuing the navigation, or otherwise continuing to use the APA's websites, you confirm that you understand and accept the terms of the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including the utilization of cookies.

×
Published Online:

For patients with medically unexplained symptoms, sometimes called somatization, there is little or no disease explanation for their numerous physical complaints ( 1 , 2 ). These patients become a problem in primary care when they embark on a quest to find an organic disease that they fear but do not have. Physicians may test for one or more organic causes—and even treat a nonexistent disease. Unnecessary laboratory testing and consultation results in increased costs and increased rates of iatrogenic complications, such as drug addiction from trial treatments of nonexistent conditions ( 3 , 4 , 5 , 6 , 7 , 8 , 9 ). Equally worrisome, physicians often ignore these patients' psychosocial distress ( 10 , 11 , 12 , 13 ). Rates of mental and physical dysfunction are high, disability and poor work records are common, relationships are poor, and personal distress prevails. Because of the high prevalence of unexplained symptoms among outpatients ( 14 , 15 , 16 , 17 , 18 ), the health care system experiences excessive costs and utilization, and harmful effects on patients may result ( 12 , 13 , 19 , 20 , 21 , 22 , 23 , 24 , 25 ).

High-use patients with somatization may account for 5%–10% of all outpatients in primary care ( 12 ). Although most such data are based on the DSM-IV diagnosis of somatoform disorder, we recently found that more than 75% of distressed, high-use patients with medically unexplained symptoms did not have either a full or an abridged DSM-IV diagnosis of somatoform disorder ( 26 ).

In a key initial study by Smith and colleagues, a consultation letter to the patient's physician from the study's research team was shown to produce improvement in costs as well as in health outcomes among patients with somatization disorder, the most severe form of somatoform disorder ( 27 , 28 ). Using a more comprehensive treatment, in a recent randomized controlled trial we found evidence that an intensive primary care intervention could be successfully conducted by primary care clinicians over 12 months ( 29 ). A cognitive-behavioral and pharmacological treatment that has a strong emphasis on the provider-patient relationship was adapted for primary care and produced a clinically significant improvement in mental health function after the 12-month intervention. Depression and satisfaction with providers also improved, physical disability decreased, antidepressant use increased, and use of addicting agents decreased ( 29 ).

As part of the trial, we sought to compare differences in health care costs and components of costs for patients who received the intervention with those who received usual care and to determine whether the treatment led to decreased health care use outside the health maintenance organization (HMO) where the study was conducted. Distinguishing the differences in components of care and utilization—not just differences in total costs—has short- and long-term implications. An increase in prescription use and office visits in the short run may reduce costly hospitalizations in the longer term ( 30 ).

Methods

Design

Details of the randomized controlled trial have been published elsewhere ( 1 , 2 , 26 , 29 , 31 , 32 ). The study was approved by institutional review boards at Michigan State University and the HMO. Members of the staff-model HMO were recruited during May 2000 through January 2002 and gave informed consent. Criteria included membership in the HMO for the previous two years, age between 18 and 65 years, no more than one visit per month to a mental health professional in the year before study entry, and eight or more physician office visits per year for the past two years. Using explicit guidelines ( 1 ), chart raters who were clinicians identified patients with medically unexplained symptoms, which was defined as having no documented organic disease to explain symptoms of at least six months' duration. Only patients with the most severe comorbid organic diseases were excluded, which left 206 patients with medically unexplained symptoms as documented in their medical charts.

These patients were randomly assigned to treatment or usual care in the HMO. The treatment protocol was deployed by four nurse practitioners, who received 84 hours of training. The protocol emphasized the provider-patient relationship and included cognitive-behavioral therapy and pharmacological management.

Cognitive-behavioral treatment involved cognitive restructuring (including symptom diaries when indicated) and operant mechanisms tailored for busy primary care providers (for example, regularly scheduled visits and medications). To maximize other aspects of treatment ( 2 ), nurse practitioners used a behaviorally defined, five-step patient-centered method to establish a positive physician-patient relationship and a three-step patient-centered method to inform and motivate patients specifically about treatment ( 33 , 34 ). Although they did not use a training manual, nurse practitioners carefully followed the training they received. This was ensured by periodic taped reviews of their interactions as well as individual supervision and group meetings every one to three weeks. In the analysis that corrected for baseline severity of mental dysfunction, no difference in outcomes by individual nurse practitioner was found.

The nurse practitioners also followed a collaborative stepped-care approach that has been useful in treating depression in primary care ( 35 ). They took full care of the patient, including management of frequent comorbid medical conditions. Rarely, for severe problems or hospital admissions, they consulted the patient's physician. Treatment focused on both mental and physical functioning and symptoms, but the primary endpoint was improved mental health functioning. Treatment included antidepressants, reduction or elimination of controlled-substance medications that were ineffective, exercise, relaxation training, physical therapy, and management of comorbid organic diseases. Most referrals were to mental health professionals, usually for improved patients who would benefit from counseling. Treatment entailed 12 scheduled patient visits with the nurse practitioner (20 minutes each) in weeks 1, 2, 3, 5, 8, 12, 16, 22, 28, 36, 44, and 52, but additional visits could occur. Telephone contact (from five to ten minutes) was scheduled between visits.

Patients in the control group received usual care from 21 HMO physicians and had their usual access to mental health professionals.

Outcomes were assessed by independent evaluators. For these patients, clinically significant improvement in the Mental Component Summary of the 36-item Short Form from the Medical Outcomes Study (SF-36) was found and reported elsewhere; in addition, depression, physical disability, and satisfaction with providers all improved, and there was an increase in use of antidepressants and a decrease in the use of addicting agents (narcotics, tranquilizers, and sleeping pills) ( 29 ).

Measurements of costs

Much of the primary care for the study participants was provided by HMO staff at the HMO facilities. However, many services were also purchased by the HMO from outside providers—including, for example, all inpatient hospital care. We used data on costs from the HMO's electronic management information system. For each participant, we used information on services delivered during the year that the individual was enrolled in the study. Costs are reported in current dollars from the time of the study.

In analyzing costs our intent was to measure the value of the resources used to deliver medical care to study participants. For services purchased by the HMO, such as outside referrals (which may have included psychiatric care), we documented the amount that the provider was paid, inclusive of patient copayments. We took this amount as our approximation of the resource costs of services. For services provided within the HMO, the management information system indicated the amount that an outside provider would have been paid for the same service. We used this information to approximate the costs of such in-house services. Because of a lack of data, we did not attempt to measure the costs of participants' time associated with receipt of services.

The intervention used nurse practitioners as primary care providers, but we calculated costs as though services were delivered by physicians. That is, the cost allocated to a particular type of visit at the HMO was treated as the same regardless of the provider type. Because nurse practitioner time has a lower cost than physician time, this approach may bias the comparison against the intervention. However, the intervention protocol requires regular meetings between nurse practitioners and physician supervisors ( 7 ) that were not separately accounted for in the cost analysis, and nurse practitioners' time spent on phone contacts with participants was also not included, because phone calls to patients were not recorded in the management information system. We believe the omission of meeting time and phone contact roughly offsets the failure to account for the lower cost of nurse practitioner time.

Although one of the criteria for inclusion in the study was that a participant expected to be a member of the HMO for at least one year after entering the study, participants who were not covered by the HMO during part of the 12-month period were included in the randomized controlled trial. However, because information on their use of services was incomplete or absent from the management information system, we excluded them (four in the treatment group and seven in usual care) from the cost analysis.

In addition to examining total costs of services, we looked at several components: costs associated with inpatient hospitalizations, all other costs (that is, other than for hospitalization), and costs for medications. We categorized medications by the National Drug Code into six groups: antidepressants, controlled-substance medications, musculoskeletal medications, pain medications, psychotropic medications (including antipsychotic, antianxiety, and alpha-agonist medications and stimulants), and others.

To capture health care utilization outside the HMO, at six- and 12-month follow-ups we asked the participants whether in the past three months they had any visits to other physicians for medical consultations, second opinions, or ongoing medical treatment; counseling of any type; visits to a chiropractor; visits to an urgent care facility; visits to an emergency room; diagnostic tests (such as blood tests and x-rays); acupuncture treatments; pain treatments; hypnosis; biofeedback sessions; or outpatient surgery or treatment that were not covered or authorized by the network. We then aggregated the corresponding out-of-pocket cost for these visits.

Statistical analysis

In addition to the 11 patients excluded for lack of cost data, six participants (three in each group) did not complete the study and were excluded from the analysis, leaving a total sample of 189 patients. Baseline characteristics of patients were compared by using the t test for continuous variables and the chi square test for categorical variables. For costs we used the Wilcoxon rank-sum test.

As is very common with health care data, our cost distributions were skewed to the right. To mitigate the influence of skewness and outlying observations, we assumed a log-normal or log-logistic distribution to model total cost. In our sample, total cost was positive for all patients. There was one extreme outlier in the control group in the distribution of total costs (primarily attributable to hospital costs of $119,202). Covariates considered for inclusion in the model of costs were age at study entry, sex, marital status, education level (16 or more years of formal schooling compared with less than 16 years), eight subscales from the SF-36 (mental health, emotional role, social functioning, vitality, general health, bodily pain, physical role, and physical functioning), score on the Center for Epidemiological Studies-Depression scale (CES-D), the state anxiety scale of the Spielberger State Anxiety Inventory (STAI), the proportion of reported medically unexplained symptoms among all symptoms, and the score on the Psychosomatic Symptom Checklist. The mental component summary and physical component summary of the SF-36 were highly correlated with the eight SF-36 subscales and were not used in variable selections. Maximum likelihood estimation methods were used, and the choice of models was based on the Akaike information criterion.

Because promoting appropriate use of antidepressants was a key aim of the multifaceted intervention and there was no difference in use of other categories of medication (such as controlled substances or musculoskeletal or pain medication), we focused on the cost of antidepressant use in the study. A substantial proportion of patients were not prescribed antidepressants and incurred no cost for antidepressants. Application of the usual statistical regression methods, such as ordinary least squares, to the whole sample or log-normal models to nonzero part of the sample raises the possibility of bias.

Because our aim was to understand the actual expenditure from a budgetary standpoint, we used a two-part model to handle the clustering of zeros ( 36 ). In the first part of the two-part model we used logistic regression to estimate the probability of positive antidepressant cost. For the second part of the two-part model, we used linear, square-root or logarithm transformations for positive costs. The square-root transformation outperformed the other specifications. In this case, the estimated mean of antidepressant costs Y combined the two parts of the model; that is, a logit model for part 1: P(Y<0|x 1 )={1+exp (-x 1β1 )} -1 and a linear predication for the transformed costs for part 2: E(Y|Y< 0,x 2 )=(x 2β2 ) 2 + σ2 , where σ2 is the conditional variance of √Y. We get E(Y|x)={(x 2β2 ) 2 + σ2 }{1+exp(-x 1β1 )} -1 . For each part of the two-part model we used stepwise selection methods to choose potential covariates from the list of covariates above. Only statistically significant covariates and the intervention dummy are reported in the final model.

Results

As shown in Table 1 , no statistically significant differences at baseline were found between participants in the treatment and control groups in gender, marital status, education level, use of outside health care, CES-D score, score on the state anxiety scale on the Spielberger STAI, proportion of medically unexplained symptoms of all reported symptoms, and the Psychosomatic Symptom Checklist score. (Data on race were not available.) The alpha was set at a more stringent level of .01 to reduce overall type I error. Age and mental and physical components of the SF-36 scores were slightly different between the two groups, but the differences were not statistically significant.

Table 1 Baseline characteristics of the patients with medically unexplained symptoms in a health maintenance organization (HMO) who were randomly assigned to an intervention group or usual care
Table 1 Baseline characteristics of the patients with medically unexplained symptoms in a health maintenance organization (HMO) who were randomly assigned to an intervention group or usual care
Enlarge table

Table 2 shows that during the year of the intervention and the year afterward a larger proportion of patients used antidepressants in the treatment group than in the control group (76% in the treatment group compared with 57% in the control group in the first year, and 73% compared with 53% in the second year). There were no statistically significant differences in the proportions of other service use.

Table 2 Components of care received in the intervention year (year 1) and one year after (year 2) by patients with medically unexplained symptoms in a health maintenance organization (HMO) who were randomly assigned to an intervention group or usual care
Table 2 Components of care received in the intervention year (year 1) and one year after (year 2) by patients with medically unexplained symptoms in a health maintenance organization (HMO) who were randomly assigned to an intervention group or usual care
Enlarge table

As shown in Table 3 , the unadjusted total cost and components of cost did not differ between the two groups. However, the simple comparisons of means could be misleading because of outliers and skewness of the data. For example, the average total cost was $815 higher for the control group, a difference that was not significant. Removing one outlier in the control group reversed the direction of the mean difference, making it $554 higher for the treatment group than the control group; however, the difference was still insignificant.

Table 3 Unadjusted cost of services in the intervention year (year 1) and one year after (year 2) for patients with medically unexplained symptoms in a health maintenance organization (HMO) who were randomly assigned to an intervention group or usual care
Table 3 Unadjusted cost of services in the intervention year (year 1) and one year after (year 2) for patients with medically unexplained symptoms in a health maintenance organization (HMO) who were randomly assigned to an intervention group or usual care
Enlarge table

Total cost

Table 4 shows the log-logistic regression results for total cost among all participants. The log-logistic model was preferred because it had lower Akaike information criterion (AIC) values than the log-normal model in both year 1 (AIC=456.11 for log-normal and AIC=447.11 for loglogistic) and year 2 (AIC=559.97 for log-normal and AIC=554.30 for log-logistic). In year 2 one patient in the control group included in the year 1 analysis left the HMO and was therefore excluded.

Table 4 Adjusted total cost by log-logistic regression, in the intervention year (year 1) and one year after (year 2) for patients with medically unexplained symptoms who were randomly assigned to an intervention group or usual care
Table 4 Adjusted total cost by log-logistic regression, in the intervention year (year 1) and one year after (year 2) for patients with medically unexplained symptoms who were randomly assigned to an intervention group or usual care
Enlarge table

Treatment was not statistically associated with total cost. Among all symptoms, the proportion of medically unexplained symptoms was negatively associated with total cost, perhaps because services for medically unexplained symptoms mostly involved laboratory or other diagnostic tests, which are less expensive than treatments for other causes, such as for organic diseases. The Psychosomatic Symptom Checklist score, on the other hand, was positively associated with total cost in the year of the study. More psychosomatic problems perhaps required more expensive care by specialists.

On the basis of the log-logistic distribution, the mean total cost was E(Y|x)=exp(x′ β )Γ(1+ σ )Γ(1- σ ),0< σ <1, where Γ is the gamma function and σ is the shape parameter. At the average level of the proportion of medically unexplained symptoms and at the average level of the Psychosomatic Symptom Checklist score, the estimated mean total cost between the two groups differed by $1,071 (95% confidence interval [CI]=-606 to 2,749). The confidence interval was calculated by the delta method.

In year 2, however, the difference in total cost between the treatment and control groups was reduced to $341 (CI=-2,113 to 2,794). This could be partly explained by the increased number of control group participants who received inpatient care in year 2. As seen in Table 2 , in year 1 both treatment and control groups had an 11% hospitalization rate. In year, 2 however, the rates were 14% for the treatment group and 20% for the control group.

Medication cost

Ideally, the intervention would lead to better management of patients with medically unexplained symptoms and to more appropriate use of all forms of health care, including hospitalizations. It appears, however, that such an effect, if it exists, cannot be estimated reliably in our experiment, because of limited sample size and other sources of variability in health care use. Therefore, we considered categories of medication use, over which the intervention might have a more direct effect.

Part 1 of the two-part model in Table 5 shows that more patients in the intervention group than in the control group were prescribed antidepressants in year 1 (odds ratio [OR]=2.43, CI=1.23–4.80) and in year 2 (OR=2.81, CI=1.42–5.58). Patients with higher scores on the Spielberger STAI were more likely to be on antidepressants, and they had spent more money on them. In year 1 patients who were female were more likely to use antidepressants, but patients with more education (more than 16 years of schooling) were less likely to do so. Among persons who used antidepressants, those with higher role-emotional scores incurred lower costs. Combining part 1 and part 2 of the two-part model, we derived the expected values of antidepressant cost. In year 1 the average antidepressant cost for the treatment group was $192 higher than for the control group (CI=41–342). This difference was statistically significant (p=.012). In year 2 the difference decreased to $123 (CI=-36 to 251) and was no longer statistically significant.

Table 5 Predicted mean antidepressant cost in two-part models in the intervention year (year 1) and one year after (year 2) for patients with medically unexplained symptoms who were randomly assigned to an intervention group or usual care
Table 5 Predicted mean antidepressant cost in two-part models in the intervention year (year 1) and one year after (year 2) for patients with medically unexplained symptoms who were randomly assigned to an intervention group or usual care
Enlarge table

Health care costs outside the HMO

At six and 12 months no difference was found between the treatment and control groups in the proportion of patients seeking health care outside of the HMO (at six months, 30 participants, or 32% in the treatment group, compared with 26 participants, or 27%, in the control group; and at 12 months, 25 participants, or 27%, in the treatment group compared with 28 participants, or 30%, in the control group). However, a reduction in the use of outside care was found for the treatment group from six months to 12 months after the intervention, whereas use increased for the control group. At 12-month follow-up, participants in the control group had spent a mean±SD of $65±$36 more out of pocket for medical services than the treatment group in the three months before the follow-up interview.

Discussion

Overall, we did not find a significant difference in total costs between the treatment and control groups. The treatment group had higher medication use and costs but a lower hospitalization rate and less use of care outside of the HMO. In an update of the Agency for Health Care Policy and Research practice guidelines for depression, Schulberg and colleagues ( 37 ) concluded that "the clinical and functional outcomes of depressed patients can be improved at a cost of $750 to $1,500 per enhanced treatment episode." Their conclusion was based on randomized controlled trials for treating major depression in primary care settings. Our study indicates similar findings for patients with medically unexplained symptoms, many of whom were depressed, in primary care practice. Our point estimate of the year 1 total cost differential was $1,071. About $200 of this differential was attributable to antidepressant use. In a previous study we found improved mental health outcomes and satisfaction for this sample of patients ( 29 ). Coupled with the finding of the smaller estimated cost differential in year 2 after the intervention, our findings indicate the potential for cost-effectiveness of the treatment strategy.

Few randomized controlled trials address the question of whether efficacious treatments reduce general health care costs or result in less loss of productivity from the societal perspective in the long term. However, our study showed less out-of-plan expense by 12 months. In addition, at six- and 12-month follow-ups, we asked each employed participant, "During the past four weeks, how many days of work did you miss due to illness?" The results indicated that the patients in the intervention group on average missed one less day at six months (1.23 days), although the difference, which was not significant (p=.08), disappeared by 12 months (.09 days, p=.90).

Some important limitations of our study should be noted. There has been doubt about the feasibility of performing economic evaluations alongside clinical trials ( 38 ). Power calculations for sample size have typically been based on clinical outcomes. Economic outcomes, and particularly those related to costs, typically exhibit more variation and other irregularities, such as missing data, skewness, or censoring. Our sample was not large enough to allow us to estimate with precision differences in total cost and components of cost attributable to treatment ( 39 ). We carried out a formal sample size analysis as follows. To test the equivalence of mean cost between the intervention and usual-care groups in a loglogistic model for cost with treatment indicator only, the null hypothesis is H 0TCδ that the relative cost is not greater than a prespecified δ . Analysis of costs in our randomized controlled trial suggested the shape parameter for the log-logistic distribution is σ =.456. To detect a 15% increase in total costs with 80% power at the level of significance of .05, the needed sample size is 546 per group. Nonetheless, we believe our results are consistent with other findings and can enrich future meta-analyses on the issue.

We are not able to estimate indirect costs from the patient's perspective: traveling costs, productivity lost due to illness, or time. However, we were able to measure the benefit of the treatment in terms of a reduction in number of lost work days and improvement in hours worked. Our findings suggest marginal benefit in reduction and improvement.

Finally, there was a one-time training cost for a nurse practitioner, involving the time of the nurse practitioner and a physician supervisor. We estimate this cost to be about $21,500 (including the cost of extra group meetings during a nurse practitioner's first year) if one nurse practitioner is trained ( 31 ) and less if more than one is trained. This cost should be divided over the number of patients the nurse practitioner sees. If a typical nurse practitioner provides care for 15 patients with medically unexplained symptoms each year for five years, the training cost amounts to less than $400 per patient per year.

Conclusions

We studied the cost implications of a treatment protocol shown to be effective for patients with medically unexplained symptoms in primary care. We did not find a statistically significant difference in total cost for the intervention group, even though more frequent physician visits and greater use of antidepressant medication were observed. It is possible that the intervention may reduce costs in the longer term, but this issue requires further study with larger samples. Coupled with findings of improved mental health outcomes found in a previous study, our results indicate that the intervention may be cost-effective.

Acknowledgments and disclosures

This study was supported by grant MH-57099 from the National Institute of Mental Health and grant HS-14206 from the Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality.

The authors report no competing interests.

Dr. Luo and Dr. Gardiner are affiliated with the Department of Epidemiology, Michigan State University, B601 West Fee Hall, East Lansing, MI 48824 (e-mail: [email protected]). Dr. Goddeeris is with the Department of Economics and Dr. Smith is with the Department of Medicine at the university.

References

1. Smith RC, Korban E, Kanj M, et al: A method for rating charts to identify and classify patients with medically unexplained symptoms. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics 73:36–42, 2004Google Scholar

2. Smith RC, Lein C, Collins C, et al: Treating patients with medically unexplained symptoms in primary care. Journal of General Internal Medicine 18:478–489, 2003Google Scholar

3. Deyo RA: Pain and public policy. New England Journal of Medicine 342:1211–1213, 2000Google Scholar

4. Cherkin D, Deyo RA: Nonsurgical hospitalization for low back pain: is it necessary? Spine 18:1728–1735, 1993Google Scholar

5. Lightfoot JRW, Luft BJ, Rahn DW, et al: Empiric parenteral antibiotic treatment of patients with fibromyalgia and fatigue and a positive serologic result for Lyme disease: a cost-effectiveness analysis. Annals of Internal Medicine 119:503–509, 1993Google Scholar

6. Fink P: Surgery and medical treatment in persistent somatizing patients. Journal of Psychosomatic Research 36:439–447, 1992Google Scholar

7. Deyo RA, Ciol MA, Cherkin DC, et al: Lumbar spinal fusion: a cohort study of complications, reoperations, and resource use in the Medicare population. Spine 1:1463–1470, 1993Google Scholar

8. Hoffman RM, Wheeler KJ, Deyo RA: Surgery for herniated lumbar discs: a literature synthesis. Journal of General Internal Medicine 8:487–496, 1993Google Scholar

9. Bass C, Benjamin S: The management of chronic somatisation. British Journal of Psychiatry 162:472–480, 1993Google Scholar

10. Bass C, Murphy M: Somatisation disorder in a British teaching hospital. British Journal of Clinical Practice 45:237–244, 1991Google Scholar

11. Escobar JI, Waitzkin H, Silver RC, et al: Abridged somatization: a study in primary care. Psychosomatic Medicine 60:466–472, 1998Google Scholar

12. DeGruy F, Columbia L, Dickinson P: Somatization disorder in a family practice. Journal of Family Practice 25:45–51, 1987Google Scholar

13. DeGruy F, Crider J, Hashimi DK, et al: Somatization disorder in a university hospital. Journal of Family Practice 25:579–584, 1987Google Scholar

14. Katon WJ, Walker EA: Medically unexplained symptoms in primary care. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 59(suppl 20):15–21, 1998Google Scholar

15. Kroenke K, Mangelsdorff AD: Common symptoms in ambulatory care: incidence, evaluation, therapy, and outcome. American Journal of Medicine 86:262–266, 1989Google Scholar

16. Barsky AJ, Borus JF: Somatization and medicalization in the era of managed care. JAMA 274:1931–1934, 1995Google Scholar

17. Hilkevitch A: Psychiatric disturbances in outpatients of a general medical outpatient clinic. International Journal of Neuropsychiatry 1:371–375, 1965Google Scholar

18. Culpan R, Davies B: Psychiatric illness at a medical and a surgical outpatient clinic. Comprehensive Psychiatry 1:228–235, 1960Google Scholar

19. Simon GE: Psychiatric disorder and functional somatic symptoms as predictors of health care use. Psychiatric Medicine 10:49–59, 1992Google Scholar

20. Katon W, Von Korff M, Lin E, et al: Distressed high utilizers of medical care: DSM-III-R diagnoses and treatment needs. General Hospital Psychiatry 12:355–362, 1990Google Scholar

21. Smith GR Jr: The course of somatization and its effects on utilization of health care resources. Psychosomatics 35:263–267, 1994Google Scholar

22. Escobar JI, Golding JM, Hough RL, et al: Somatization in the community: relationship to disability and use of services. American Journal of Public Health 77:837–840, 1987Google Scholar

23. Escobar JI, Canino G: Unexplained physical complaints: psychopathology and epidemiological correlates. British Journal of Psychiatry Supplement 4:24–27, 1989Google Scholar

24. Escobar JI, Swartz M, Rubio-Stipec M, et al: Medically unexplained symptoms distribution, risk factors, and comorbidity, in Current Concepts of Somatization: Research and Clinical Perspectives. Edited by Kirmayer LJ, Robbins JM. Washington, DC, American Psychiatric Press, 1991Google Scholar

25. Escobar JI, Burnam MA, Karno M, et al: Somatization in the community. Archives of General Psychiatry 44:713–718, 1987Google Scholar

26. Smith RC, Gardiner JC, Lyles JS, et al: Exploration of DSM-IV criteria in primary care patients with medically unexplained symptoms. Psychosomatic Medicine 67:123–129, 2005Google Scholar

27. Smith GR Jr, Monson RA, Ray DC: Psychiatric consultation in somatization disorder: a randomized controlled study. New England Journal of Medicine 314:1407–1413, 1986Google Scholar

28. Smith GR Jr, Rost K, Kashner TM: A trial of the effect of a standardized psychiatric consultation on health outcomes and costs in somatizing patients. Archives of General Psychiatry 52:238–243, 1995Google Scholar

29. Smith RC, Lyles JS, Gardiner JC, et al: Primary care clinicians treat patients with medically unexplained symptoms: a randomized controlled trial. Journal of General Internal Medicine 21:671–677, 2006Google Scholar

30. Bechdolf A, Kohn D, Knost B, et al: A randomized comparison of group cognitive-behavioural therapy and group psychoeducation in acute patients with schizophrenia: outcome at 24 months. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica 112:173–179, 2005Google Scholar

31. Lyles JS, Hodges A, Collins C, et al: Using nurse practitioners to implement an intervention in primary care for high-utilizing patients with medically unexplained symptoms. General Hospital Psychiatry 25:63–73, 2003Google Scholar

32. Lein C, Collins C, Lyles JS, et al: Building research relationships with managed care organizations: issues and strategies. Families, Systems and Health 21:205–214, 2003Google Scholar

33. Smith RC: Patient-Centered Interviewing: An Evidence-Based Method. Philadelphia, Lippincott Williams and Wilkins, 2002Google Scholar

34. Smith RC, Lyles JS, Mettler J, et al: The effectiveness of intensive training for residents in interviewing: a randomized, controlled study. Annals of Internal Medicine 128:118–126, 1998Google Scholar

35. Katon W, Von Korff M, Lin E, et al: Stepped collaborative care for primary care patients with persistent symptoms of depression: a randomized trial. Archives of General Psychiatry. 46:1109–1115, 1999Google Scholar

36. Dow WH, Norton EC: Choosing between and interpreting the Heckit and two-part models for corner solutions. Health Services and Outcomes Research Methodology 4:5–18, 2003Google Scholar

37. Schulberg HC, Katon W, Simon GE, et al: Treating major depression in primary care practice: an update of the Agency for Health Care Policy and Research Practice Guidelines. Archives of General Psychiatry 55:1121–1127, 1998Google Scholar

38. Gray AM, Marshall M, Lockwood A, et al: Problems in conducting economic evaluations alongside clinical trials: lessons from a study of case management for people with mental disorders. British Journal of Psychiatry 170:47–52, 1997Google Scholar

39. Gardiner JC, Huebner M, Jetton J, et al: Power and sample assessments for tests of hypotheses on cost-effectiveness ratios. Health Economics 9:227–234, 2000Google Scholar