The American Psychiatric Association (APA) has updated its Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including with new information specifically addressed to individuals in the European Economic Area. As described in the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, this website utilizes cookies, including for the purpose of offering an optimal online experience and services tailored to your preferences.

Please read the entire Privacy Policy and Terms of Use. By closing this message, browsing this website, continuing the navigation, or otherwise continuing to use the APA's websites, you confirm that you understand and accept the terms of the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including the utilization of cookies.

×
Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ps.202100253

Abstract

Clinician bias is a contributor to health care inequities, but research on racial-ethnic bias among mental health professionals, especially toward minoritized youths, is limited. This column describes two studies involving mental health clinicians in schools, where most youths access mental health services. Study 1 used a mixed-methods approach to identify stereotypes about Black and Latinx youths salient to clinicians (e.g., academic failure; anger and aggression). In study 2, the authors developed four Implicit Association Tests to assess clinicians’ implicit prejudice and stereotyping of Black and Latinx youths and found pro-White and anti-Black/Latinx bias at levels similar to those of other health care providers and the general population.

HIGHLIGHTS

  • Clinician bias has long been identified as a key contributing factor to health care inequities, but previous research has rarely studied bias among mental health professionals.

  • School-based mental health professionals associated distinct stereotypes, including academic failure, with both Black and Latinx youths and more frequently associated anger and aggression with Black youths.

  • School-based mental health professionals’ scores on the Implicit Association Test showed pro-White, anti-Black, and anti-Latinx biases toward school-age youths at levels similar to those of other health care professionals’ and to the general population’s biases toward racial-ethnic minoritized groups.

Despite having rates of psychiatric disorder similar to those of their non-Hispanic White counterparts, Black and Latinx youths receive a lower quantity and lower quality of mental health services and have critically higher rates of unmet need (1). Research has shown persistent disparities in diagnosis, service access, and the quantity and quality of treatment, all of which lead Black and Latinx youths to have worse outcomes in many domains beyond mental health (1).

The broader health services literature points to health care providers’ bias as a major contributor to persistent disparities (2). Clinician bias includes prejudice (both positive and negative evaluations) and stereotyping (traits associated with a given social group). Prejudice and stereotyping can manifest as implicit biases that are relatively less conscious or controllable than explicit or overt biases (2). Implicit bias has been shown to negatively affect clinical decision making and patient-provider relationships, both of which can lead to downstream disparate outcomes for minoritized populations compared with non-Hispanic Whites (2). Because implicit biases may influence behavior outside of one’s awareness or conscious control, their impacts may be present in almost any interaction between health care providers and patients belonging to minoritized groups (3). Thus, understanding clinicians’ knowledge of the specific stereotypes of minoritized patient groups can inform the development of interventions designed to reduce discrimination.

Although there is an established literature on health care providers’ implicit racial bias (4), only a few of those studies included mental health professionals, a group that has been shown to be more empathic (5) and, by extension, possibly less biased than other health care professionals (empathy has been shown to reduce bias [6]). However, bias among mental health clinicians may have a greater influence on producing disparate outcomes, compared with bias among other health care providers, given the field’s limited adoption of treatment algorithms or standardized guidelines, which could mitigate the impact of clinician bias (7). The literature also primarily focuses on bias toward Black adult patients, with only a few studies examining bias toward youths or other minoritized populations (4). Previous research with non–health care populations (e.g., teachers, undergraduates) has found that adults have implicit racial bias toward youths as young as preschool age (8).

School is the most common setting for youth mental health service delivery and one that has been shown to ameliorate some service disparities (e.g., access), while others persist (e.g., treatment engagement) (9). No studies have examined school-based mental health clinicians’ racial bias toward youths. Improving understanding of bias among school-based clinicians is an important step in addressing pediatric mental health service disparity in a setting where youths are most likely to access mental health care. This column describes two novel studies of school-based clinicians’ knowledge of stereotypes and implicit bias toward Black and Latinx youths.

Study 1

As part of a series of studies contributing to the development of an online implicit-bias intervention for school-based mental health clinicians, this study aimed to identify stereotypes of Black and Latinx youths known to school clinicians.

Methods

A geographically diverse U.S. sample of school-based clinicians (N=42) was recruited via relevant professional Listservs. Demographically representative of school-based clinicians nationally, the sample was predominantly White (N=34, 81%) and female (N=40, 95%), with 10% (N=4) identifying as Black and 7% (N=3) as Latinx. Following the approach of Bean and colleagues (3), we asked participants to complete a brief online survey that prompted them to “think about conversations [they’ve] overheard or had with [their] colleagues” and identify “behaviors or characteristics [they] think school mental health professionals most commonly associate with” Black youths and, separately, with Latinx youths. For each minoritized group, participants were first asked to generate free responses and then to select the top three from a list of 14 characteristics, seven of which were previously documented in the literature (i.e., aggressive, angry, delinquent, gang-involved, low IQ, poor academics, and unmotivated) and six of which were positive or neutral attributes included for balance (i.e., conscientious, easygoing, hardworking, high achieving, natural leader, passive). The same list of characteristics was used for Black and for Latinx youths.

Following established methodology for thematic coding of qualitative data, participants’ free responses were systematically reviewed and coded by three independent coders blinded to the identified minoritized group. This process (delineated in the online supplement) reduced each free response to nominal data that indicated whether high-frequency stereotypes (i.e., aggression and anger, academic failure, delinquency, lack of motivation) were identified by each participant. Cross-category coding, following the methods of Bean and colleagues (3), was then completed, such that if a participant indicated a stereotype either in the free response or in the top three multiple-choice selections, the response was categorized as a stereotype for the relevant minoritized group.

Results

Eighty-one percent (N=34) of participants identified academic failure as the characteristic most frequently associated with Black and with Latinx youths. Whereas 76% (N=32) identified anger/aggression as a stereotype for Black youths, only 29% (N=12) identified this characteristic as a stereotype for Latinx youths (χ2=18.38, df=42, N=1, p<0.01). The second- and third-most frequently identified stereotypes for Latinx youths were being unmotivated (N=26, 62%) and delinquency/rule breaking (N=20, 48%). These two stereotypes were fourth- and third-most frequently identified for Black youths (N=19, 45%; N=23, 55%, respectively) at rates not significantly different from those of Latinx youths. (See the online supplement for a graph and table of chi-square analysis results.)

Study 2

Based on the findings of study 1, four distinct Implicit Association Tests (IATs) were developed to assess school-based mental health clinicians’ implicit prejudice and stereotyping of Black and Latinx youths relative to non-Hispanic White youths. The IAT, the most well-established measure of implicit biases (10), is a computerized categorization task that measures the relative strengths of associations by examining the speed with which people assign images and words to categories. For example, people who have stronger pro-White or anti-Black biases would be faster at pairing White people with good things (e.g., words of positive attributes and connotations) and Black people with bad things (e.g., words of negative attributes and connotations) than the reverse. This difference in reaction times can be computed as a D score (i.e., IAT difference scores); in this example, positive scores indicate a pro-White or anti-Black bias, negative scores indicate a pro-Black or anti-White bias, and scores of 0 or near 0 indicate no bias.

Methods

A new sample of school-based mental health clinicians (N=58) was recruited for study 2 by using the same procedures as study 1. The sample was again predominantly White (N=43, 74%) and female (N=52, 90%), with 19% (N=11) identifying as Black and 9% (N=5) identifying as Latinx. Participants completed all study procedures online via a secure data collection platform (Qualtrics), including the four IATs; a questionnaire collecting demographic characteristics; and the Bias Awareness Scale, a four-item questionnaire that measures awareness of and concern for one’s own implicit bias (11). Participants were asked about their awareness of bias toward Black and Latinx youths separately, yielding two distinct bias awareness scores.

The original Black-White IAT and the more recently developed Latinx IATs all use sets of adult faces. To the best of our knowledge, no previously published IATs have used faces of Black or Latinx youths, representing the full developmental range of school-age youths (i.e., kindergarten through 12th grade). Thus, we developed two IATs to assess implicit prejudice (i.e., biases toward Black vs. White youths and toward Latinx vs. White youths) and two IATs to assess implicit stereotyping (i.e., assessing defiance/obedience as being associated with Black vs. White youths and academic failure/academic success as being associated with Latinx vs. White youths) on the basis of the two most frequently identified stereotypes found in study 1. This process was completed in accordance with established best practices for IAT development (see the online supplement for more details, including a complete reference list relevant to the methodology and the list of photo stimuli and attributes used).

Results

All four IATs performed as expected, with adequate internal consistency (α=0.71–0.91) and low rates of dropped trials (5%–6%) and dropped participants (one per IAT) due to invalid responding (e.g., too slow, too fast, or making too many errors). All four IATs generated average D scores between 0.30 and 0.42, indicative of implicit prejudice or stereotypes favoring White over Black and Latinx youths. These results (Cohen’s d=0.81) were similar to those previously reported among health care providers (4) and among the general population toward minoritized racial-ethnic groups (12). All four IATs were positively correlated with each other (suggesting that they captured clinician bias in similar ways) and negatively correlated with participants’ self-reported awareness of their own implicit bias, as expected (i.e., those who demonstrated greater implicit bias reported less personal awareness and concern for implicit bias). (See the online supplement for psychometrics and correlation tables.)

Discussion

These two studies on school mental health clinicians’ knowledge of stereotype and implicit bias contribute foundational knowledge toward developing interventions to address implicit racial bias among mental health professionals working in schools—the setting where youths are most likely to access mental health services (9).

Knowledge of stereotypes is a critical antecedent to biased care because stereotype knowledge can cause biased behavior even when clinicians do not endorse those stereotypes. Findings from study 1 suggest that most school mental health clinicians are aware of stereotypes about Black and Latinx youths. Several stereotypes, including academic failure, delinquency/rule breaking, and being unmotivated, were attributed roughly equally to both Black and Latinx youths, but anger/aggression was clearly more often identified for Black than for Latinx youths. These stereotypes shed light on putative mechanisms through which clinician bias can lead to care disparities. For example, implicit associations of Black youths with anger and aggression may lead to overidentification of oppositional defiant and conduct disorders and underidentification of other diagnostic explanations of noncompliant behavior, such as attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Similarly, if Latinx and Black youths’ disengagement from school is stereotypically attributed to academic failure, lack of motivation, or delinquency, other potential contributors such as learning disabilities, depression, or anxiety could go underidentified and unsupported.

The results of study 2 not only substantiate the presence of implicit bias among mental health professionals, similar to that of other health care providers, but also demonstrate that those biases apply to youths, which is consistent with previous findings regarding other youth-oriented professionals (8). Study 2 also provided four new IATs to support the study of implicit racial-ethnic biases toward youths (available from corresponding author on request). The IATs comparing Latinx with non-Hispanic White individuals were especially novel, given that previous studies of implicit racial-ethnic bias focused predominantly on Black-White comparisons. Given the possible negative downstream impacts of implicit bias on minoritized youths, the availability of these research tools allows the field to not only better detect implicit bias but to more effectively measure the impact of interventions to reduce implicit bias.

The small samples in both studies limit the generalizability of findings, such that replication is needed before firm conclusions can be drawn. Commonly cited limitations of the IAT are also relevant, particularly those related to the IAT being a measure of relative bias (e.g., pro-White/anti-Black) as opposed to assessing bias about a single construct. In other words, although we know that study 2 found that school-based clinicians held stronger implicit associations of Black and Latinx youths with negativity, defiance, and academic failure relative to White youths, we cannot make inferences about the strength of these associations in absolute terms.

Conclusions

Together, these studies represent a first examination of school mental health clinicians’ knowledge of stereotypes and implicit racial bias involving youths. They provide important foundational knowledge and research tools (new IATs) to better understand how clinician bias contributes to biased care and disparate outcomes in youth mental health.

Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, University of Washington School of Medicine, Seattle (Liu, McRee, Coifman, Lyon); Department of Psychology, University of Arizona, Tucson (Stone); Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Washington University in St. Louis, St. Louis (Lai); Department of Psychology, Pennsylvania State University, State College (Yu).
Send correspondence to Dr. Liu (). Michael Mensah, M.D., M.P.H., Lucy Ogbu-Nwobodo, M.D., M.S., and Ruth S. Shim, M.D., M.P.H., are editors of this column.

This work was funded by the National Institute of Mental Health (grants R34MH109605 and R34MH109605-S1).

Dr. Lai is a consultant and the chair of the Scientific Advisory Board with Project Implicit. The other authors report no financial relationships with commercial interests.

References

1 Alegria M, Vallas M, Pumariega AJ: Racial and ethnic disparities in pediatric mental health. Child Adolesc Psychiatr Clin N Am 2010; 19:759–774Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

2 Zestcott CA, Blair IV, Stone J: Examining the presence, consequences, and reduction of implicit bias in health care: a narrative review. Group Process Intergroup Relat 2016; 19:528–542Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

3 Bean MG, Focella ES, Covarrubias R, et al.: Documenting nursing and medical students’ stereotypes about Hispanic and American Indian patients. J Health Disparities Res Pract 2014; 7:14MedlineGoogle Scholar

4 Maina IW, Belton TD, Ginzberg S, et al.: A decade of studying implicit racial/ethnic bias in healthcare providers using the implicit association test. Soc Sci Med 2018; 199:219–229Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

5 Santamaría-García H, Baez S, García AM, et al.: Empathy for others’ suffering and its mediators in mental health professionals. Sci Rep 2017; 7:1–13Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

6 Whitford DK, Emerson AM: Empathy intervention to reduce implicit bias in pre-service teachers. Psychol Rep 2019; 122:670–688Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

7 Sabin JA, Rivara FP, Greenwald AG: Physician implicit attitudes and stereotypes about race and quality of medical care. Med Care 2008; 46:678–685Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

8 Gilliam WS, Maupin AN, Reyes CR, et al: Do Early Educators’ Implicit Biases Regarding Sex and Race Relate to Behavior Expectations and Recommendations of Preschool Expulsions and Suspensions? A Research Study Brief. New Haven, CT, Yale University, Child Study Center, 2016Google Scholar

9 Duong MT, Bruns EJ, Lee K, et al.: Rates of mental health service utilization by children and adolescents in schools and other common service settings: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Adm Policy Ment Health 2021; 48:420–439Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

10 Greenwald AG, Brendl M, Cai H, et al.: The Implicit Association Test at Age 20: What Is Known and What Is Not Known About Implicit Bias. Seattle, University of Washington, 2020. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/bf97cGoogle Scholar

11 Perry SP, Murphy MC, Dovidio JF: Modern prejudice: subtle, but unconscious? The role of bias awareness in Whites’ perceptions of personal and others’ biases. J Exp Soc Psychol 2015; 61:64–78CrossrefGoogle Scholar

12 Nosek BA, Smyth FL, Hansen JJ, et al.: Pervasiveness and correlates of implicit attitudes and stereotypes. Eur Rev Soc Psychol 2007; 18:36–88CrossrefGoogle Scholar