The American Psychiatric Association (APA) has updated its Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including with new information specifically addressed to individuals in the European Economic Area. As described in the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, this website utilizes cookies, including for the purpose of offering an optimal online experience and services tailored to your preferences.

Please read the entire Privacy Policy and Terms of Use. By closing this message, browsing this website, continuing the navigation, or otherwise continuing to use the APA's websites, you confirm that you understand and accept the terms of the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including the utilization of cookies.

×
ArticleFull Access

Barriers to the Appropriate Clinical Use of Medications That Improve the Cognitive Deficits of Schizophrenia

Published Online:

The next treatment advance in psychopharmacology may be the development of medications to treat the cognitive deficits of schizophrenia. Individuals with schizophrenia show a generalized cognitive impairment ( 1 , 2 , 3 ) in addition to deficits in global verbal memory and learning, attention, speed of visuomotor processing, and word fluency ( 4 , 5 , 6 ). Cognitive deficits are present at illness onset, stable over time ( 7 ), and relatively independent of other symptom domains ( 8 ). The cognitive deficits associated with schizophrenia ( 9 ) appear to be important determinants of functional outcomes ( 10 , 11 ), and it is hoped that treatments that enhance cognition might lessen the disability associated with schizophrenia ( 12 ). For these reasons, the National Institute of Mental Health has funded two innovative programs—Measurement and Treatment Research to Improve Cognition in Schizophrenia (MATRICS) ( 13 ) and Treatment Units for Research on Neurocognition in Schizophrenia ( 14 )—to accelerate the development of novel medications to improve the cognitive deficits of schizophrenia. Drug developers may identify cognition-enhancing compounds within the next few years and market new medications within the decade.

Clinicians will face unique challenges in prescribing medications to treat cognitive deficits of schizophrenia. Unlike hallucinations and delusions, cognitive deficits are not monitored in a typical clinic visit. With schizophrenia, aspects of thinking do not present as obvious impairments, and patients do not usually complain about them. Indeed, even experts describe the cognitive deficits of schizophrenia in a wide variety of ways, comparing them with thought disorders, social awkwardness, and disability ( 15 ). Moreover, the tools used in neuropsychology research cannot be easily translated into clinical language. Clinicians may find it difficult to determine whether cognition-enhancing medications are having beneficial effects on their patients' cognition and functioning ( 16 ).

Active efforts to accelerate innovation in novel therapeutic areas call for parallel efforts to prepare for innovation in practice ( 17 , 18 ). Clinicians in routine clinical settings incorporate evidence-based practices slowly and inconsistently into usual care ( 19 ). The persistent gap between real-world prescribing patterns and the evidence base in psychiatry ( 20 , 21 ) may indicate that clinical research provides inadequate guidance for practitioners in usual settings. While drugs are in development, it may be possible to anticipate what clinicians will need to appropriately incorporate new medications into everyday practice. Novel treatments can then be introduced along with the actionable evidence and tools clinicians need to prescribe them effectively and appropriately ( 22 ). In this case, strategizing how cognitive deficits will be clinically operationalized is crucial to planning evidence-based dissemination of new medications.

This article reviews neuropsychological evidence on cognition and functioning in schizophrenia. As it is currently organized, this evidence may not be sufficient to guide clinicians in the selection of patients, clinical assessment of cognitive deficits, and appropriate monitoring of the effectiveness of new medications for cognitive deficits. Three barriers to the appropriate clinical use of new medications are discussed. First, evidence on cognition in schizophrenia relies on laboratory constructs that are not equivalent to everyday skills and behaviors; second, the cognitive deficits in schizophrenia are heterogeneous and subtle, such that patients and providers may not readily identify or monitor them; and third, changes in patients' functional status may be difficult to measure and difficult to accurately attribute to medication effects. Each aspect may make it difficult for psychiatrists to define the clinical utility of cognition-enhancing medications and to prescribe medications effectively to the appropriate patients. These barriers may undercut the improvements in real-world outcomes that cognitive innovations could provide. Suggestions for addressing each barrier are presented.

Laboratory constructs versus real-world skills

Neuropsychological constructs of cognition are not the same as everyday notions of thinking, planning, and reasoning. The skills assessed in neurocognitive tests are narrower than everyday tasks that people perform in the course of a day. In trail-making tests, participants connect numbers and letters in alternating order (1-A-2-B-3-C, up to 13-M). Other tests measure the speed with which respondents locate target symbols or name colors ( 23 ). Memory and working memory tests include assessment of word learning or recollection of digits or other material for brief spans. Tests of visual learning and memory assess recognition of faces or family scenes or ask respondents to reproduce a line drawing. Other tests, such as the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, assess a person's ability to flexibly respond to changing cues in a sorting task. Tests of social cognition may ask respondents to rate the intensity of emotion expressed on an actor's face. The respondent's effort affects performance on neurocognitive tests ( 24 ).

Few data are available to clarify how clinical behaviors relate to the specific skills assessed on neurocognitive tests ( 25 ). Many neuropsychological tests have moderate ecological validity compared with everyday cognitive functioning ( 26 , 27 ), but much of the data on ecological validity are gathered from patients with moderate to severe brain injuries, whose cognitive deficits are more severe than in schizophrenia. In a study of neurological patients and a control group, measures of executive function were significantly predictive of relatives' or caregivers' reports of dysexecutive problems in everyday life ( 28 ), yet a replication study showed modest correlations between these measures ( 29 ). When tests of memory were compared with clinicians' reports of day-to-day memory function in adults with brain injury ( 30 ), correspondence varied according to the severity of memory impairment and the memory test used. More ecologically valid neurocognitive assessment strategies, such as highly engaging tests that assess metacognitive capacities like self-knowledge ( 25 ), are in development.

Limitations in ecological validity data do not indicate that cognition in everyday life is entirely discrepant from that measured in the laboratory, but it does mean that clinicians may not know whether patients' behaviors reflect laboratory-identified deficits that should be treated with a cognition-enhancing medication. Individuals with schizophrenia show an impairment in their ability to make social judgments from facial expressions ( 31 ) and have deficits in the recognition of familiar faces ( 32 ) in computerized tests of social cognition. Yet these deficits do not lead to obvious difficulties in patients' ability to read a speaker's emotion or recognize faces ( 33 ). In fact, patients with paranoia may be particularly adept at detecting subtle emotional clues that validate delusional thinking ( 34 ). Problems getting to appointments or remembering medications may be caused by multiple noncognitive factors, such as lack of social supports or funds, poor insight, or behavioral disorganization.

Efficacy trials for registration of promising compounds will generate robust data about changes in laboratory constructs ( 13 , 35 , 36 ). The seven domains of cognition included in the MATRICS cognitive outcome battery (working memory, attention and vigilance, verbal learning and memory, visual memory and learning, speed of processing, reasoning and problem solving, and social cognition) ( 37 ) were not chosen to represent clinically obvious aspects of cognition, nor do they necessarily mark the cognitive problems that most trouble people with schizophrenia. And although cognition is usually discussed as a general capacity ( 4 ), the relationship between separable domains of cognition ( 23 ) and general aptitude is an area of controversy in the field.

Structured assessments versus clinician and patient assessments

Cognitive deficits are not accurately assessed through patient self-report or "clinical eye"; neither matches the results of laboratory tests. In one study ( 38 ), clinicians judged memory to be normal in 20% to 40% of psychiatric patients despite substantial impairment on objective tests. Clinical judgment differed from objective measures of other domains 33% to 53% of the time. In another study ( 39 ), clinicians showed moderately good sensitivity in differentiating patients with schizophrenia and intact cognition from those with impaired cognition, but specificity rates were low. Forty percent of persons with schizophrenia who showed impaired attention and memory on neuropsychological tests did not perceive that they had a cognitive problem, and patients were less accurate in perceiving that their cognitive skills were intact. Clinicians and patients agreed at the rate of chance. Patients' cognitive complaints do not match objective scores. Prouteau and colleagues ( 40 ) found that complaints of poor attention were associated with poor visual memory and planning. Finally, patients' reports on a brief rating scale (the Schizophrenia Cognition Rating Scale, or SCoRS, and the Clinical Global Impression of Cognition in Schizophrenia, or CGI-CogS) were not identical to interviewers' or caregivers' reports, nor to other measures of cognition or functioning ( 41 ; Ventura J, Cienfuegos A, Boxer O, et al., unpublished manuscript, 2006). The discrepancy is not limited to schizophrenia; cognitive complaints in other patient populations do not strictly correlate with objective measures ( 42 , 43 , 44 , 45 ).

The subtlety and specificity of the cognitive deficits identified on neurocognitive tests are difficult to describe in clinical terms. These deficits are less pronounced than those of dementia, developmental delay, or mental retardation, and their upper and lower boundaries are indistinct. A small subgroup of patients with schizophrenia perform in the neuropsychologically normal range ( 46 , 47 , 48 , 49 ). In a meta-analysis, 78% of patients with schizophrenia scored below the median on a test of global verbal memory, but only 61% scored below the median on a test of block design ( 4 ). (The median is defined as the score within a distribution where 50% of individuals score above and 50% score below.) Finally, heterogeneity has been demonstrated in individual's cognitive profiles ( 50 ), including during a first episode of psychosis ( 51 ), and many individuals with schizophrenia retain intellectual skills ( 52 , 53 ).

Clinicians will need to measure cognitive skills, and brief rating scales are available for doing so. The Cognitive Screening Instrument for Schizophrenia, a ten-minute test, is being developed and validated (Purdon S, personal communication, 2006). Rating scales include the CGI-CogS, a 30-minute interview-rated scale that gathers input from patient and caregiver ( 54 ); the Brief Cognitive Assessment ( 55 ), a 15-minute scale used by interviewers; and the SCoRS, which is a ten-minute clinician-rated scale that measures patients' and caregivers' impressions of cognitive performance (Ventura et al., unpublished manuscript, 2006). Briefer versions of the cognitive battery used in clinical trials include the Brief Assessment of Cognition in Schizophrenia, which assesses several important domains of cognition in schizophrenia and takes 35 minutes to complete and score ( 56 ). The Repeatable Battery for the Assessment of Neuropsychological Status is also a brief cognitive battery that does not assess the most important domains of cognition in schizophrenia ( 12 ) but may be more suited to the severe cognitive deficits seen in dementia ( 57 , 58 ). Data describing scales' correlations with the objective measures of cognition, patients' complaints, clinicians' impressions, and real-world functioning are available for some rating scales ( 41 , 55 ).

Although these scales are reliable and take only minutes to administer, efforts to encourage clinicians to rely on rating scales in clinical decision making are not usually successful ( 59 , 60 ). Clinicians use clinical judgment to guide most routine clinical decisions ( 61 ). In addition, education about cognition may present clinicians with ambiguous messages about the role of rating scales. Continuing medical education materials on cognition in schizophrenia emphasize that cognitive dysfunction is present "in varying degrees (often severe) in the majority of patients with schizophrenia'' ( 62 ) and that all domains are affected. Cognition and functioning tend to be conflated in these materials ( 63 ). These messages suggest that all patients with schizophrenia would benefit from a cognition-enhancing medication. In fact, whether medications will be beneficial to all patients, whether global cognitive performance will improve, and whether functioning will change are important empirical questions. Colloquial usages of "cognition" and "functioning" may erroneously imply that there would be no need to measure cognitive deficits with more psychometric precision.

Finally, the lack of agreement between clinicians, patients, and laboratory measures on the treatment target may make it difficult for prescribers to appropriately identify patients who need cognitive enhancers. Successful treatment depends on three-way agreement between families, clinicians, and patients. Clinicians may not be able to detect changes in cognition caused by the medications, or they may perceive cognitive changes that cannot be verified on tests. Patients' reports of improvement or deterioration may not be substantiated with rating scales, and their motivation for adherence may be reduced. Clinicians may also have difficulty estimating the value of cognition medications against costs, side effects, and drug interactions. Overuse of medications through inadequate treatment targeting or underuse of medications for patients who could benefit from them ( 64 , 65 ) are possible outcomes.

Use of functional outcomes to assess cognitive changes

The fundamental reason to prescribe cognition-enhancing medications is to improve patients' daily lives. It is hoped that cognitive enhancement will lead to improvements in such outcomes as overall well-being, employment, activities of daily living, and social engagement ( 66 ). Yet most of the data on cognition and functioning are correlational, associating a discrete aspect of cognition with a discrete aspect of functioning ( 67 ). Scores on tests of vigilance, for instance, have been correlated with social problem solving ( 68 ) and with independent living ( 69 ). Associative visual memory has been correlated with autonomy in activities of daily living ( 70 ). Visual memory and working memory have been shown to explain variance in work functioning ( 11 ). The effect sizes of the relationships between cognitive domains and functional constructs usually fall in the modest or medium range ( 25 ). The association between neurocognition and quality of life appears mixed ( 70 , 71 , 72 , 73 ). It is not clear whether it is most accurate to view specific domains of cognition as correlates of specific aspects of functioning or to view the two as more generally interrelated ( 74 ).

Multiple mediating factors link cognition to real-world functioning. Negative symptoms, specific cognitive deficits, environmental factors, age, and illness duration all make varying contributions to patients' community and social functioning ( 9 , 75 ). For instance, working memory and negative symptoms explained most of the variance in daily problem solving in one study ( 76 ), and verbal memory, processing speed and attention, and negative symptoms predicted outcome in another ( 9 ). Poor verbal memory predicted impairment in recreational activities, whereas attention and negative symptoms were predictive of work performance ( 9 ). Social factors, such as prior work experience, education level, social functioning, and economic circumstances, influence work outcomes ( 77 ).

The most distal, real-world outcomes that clinicians might seek to change have the least strong correlations with specific cognitive deficits. Cognitive skills predict improvement in work quality ( 78 , 79 , 80 , 81 ) among patients in work rehabilitation, but domains of cognition have little correlation with more distal outcomes, such as type of employment ( 81 , 82 ). In a study of persons with schizophrenia in India, neurocognition did not relate significantly to current employment or work performance; negative symptoms and social functioning, however, were significant ( 83 ). Yet the employment rate for those with schizophrenia in India was approximately 70% ( 84 ), compared with less than 15% in developed countries ( 85 , 86 ). For some patients, local conditions and social factors may exert a much stronger effect on functioning than cognitive deficits.

Subtle shifts in functioning that are attributable to cognitive enhancers may be difficult for clinicians to detect, because improvements in functioning would occur gradually ( 10 ). The Global Assessment of Functioning scale (GAF) has sufficient validity in single evaluations ( 87 ) but insufficient reliability as a measure of change for an individual patient ( 88 ). Patients' self-reports of functioning often diverge from objective measures ( 12 , 89 ). Proxy or performance-based measures have been developed for use in clinical trials ( 10 , 90 ). These measures require persons to perform a simulated skill, such as paying a bill ( 91 ), preparing a meal, or responding to a simulated social interaction ( 92 ). Adequate performance on a proxy measure does not ensure that a patient can perform the skill in everyday life ( 93 ), and because proxy tests rely on contrived settings, they are not appropriate for routine clinical use.

More research on mediators between cognition and functioning may inform clinical assessment strategies. Knowledge of grocery-shopping skills has been described as a mediator between cognitive skill and grocery-shopping performance, and measures of knowledge of functional skills could be used by clinicians ( 94 ). Intermediate functional measures, such as the performance-based outcome measures, could be completed by ancillary staff, such as occupational therapists. These measures may be more strongly correlated with neuropsychological tests than brief cognitive rating scales designed for clinicians to use ( 41 ). Ecologically valid cognitive test batteries, such as those that assess metacognition, may provide clues to clinically meaningful assessment strategies. For instance, metacognition appears to be a mediator between objective cognitive deficits and poor insight ( 95 ). Recognizable clinical symptoms that can be traced directly to cognitive deficits could help clinicians direct treatments.

Discussion and conclusions

This article explains why attempts to accelerate therapeutic innovation can be coupled with efforts to prepare clinicians for the introduction of novel treatments. Accelerating the development of medications for cognition is a high priority, but prescribing cognition-enhancing medications effectively may be a complex clinical task. Constructs are not equivalent to daily activities or everyday notions of thinking and living, and data are not available to describe how cognitive domains correlate to the phenomena seen in the clinic. Clinicians' and patients' assessments often do not match objective assessments. Functional aspects of patients' lives are determined on multiple levels and are difficult for clinicians to detect and measure. If these barriers prevent appropriate selection of patients for treatment with cognition-enhancing medications and complicate the monitoring of responses to such medications, positive findings from efficacy trials of cognition-enhancing compounds may be difficult to translate into real-world improvements in outcomes.

Prescribing patterns for medications to treat Alzheimer's disease show the importance of addressing clinicians' informational needs ( 96 ). Specialists and internists differ widely in prescribing rates for cholinesterase inhibitors ( 97 ). Measures used in clinical trials are not directly applicable to clinical practice ( 98 , 99 , 100 ). Perhaps because clinicians have difficulty in assessing the efficacy of medications for Alzheimer's, both overuse and underuse have been described. Treatment guidelines are available, yet factors such as economic concerns and family preference influence prescribing. A study from Germany determined that more than 50% of clinicians prescribing cholinesterase inhibitors did not expect any positive therapeutic effect; a positive effect from the drug was expected by a minority (18%–28%) of prescribers ( 97 ). Given the similarities between the two treatment targets, cognitive enhancers for schizophrenia could be as difficult to appropriately prescribe.

Suggestions can be offered. Neuroscientific constructs of cognition and functioning ought not be conflated with everyday notions of cognition and functioning. Whereas cognition is used colloquially to indicate a general capacity to reason and process information and functioning is equated to real-world outcomes, the data are organized around discrete constructs and domains. The conflation of everyday notions with neurocognitive constructs ( 62 , 63 ) may make it difficult for clinicians to use the evidence based to guide prescribing, yet the conflation could facilitate the marketing of cognition-enhancing medications ( 101 ). More data will be needed to clarify the clinical manifestations of the deficits identified in laboratory tests of cognition. Clinicians can be educated that cognitive deficits can be reliably measured but, like abnormal cardiac conduction, cannot be accurately assessed in a routine clinical interview.

The Food and Drug Administration has suggested that a compound approved as a cognitive enhancer in schizophrenia will need to improve scores on a cognitive battery and a second measure with clinical face validity ( 23 , 102 ). However, these "co-primary" measures—patients' reports, clinicians' assessments, or proxy functioning—all have limitations as described above. Co-primary outcome measures in clinical trials may give clinicians useful information about cognitive drugs' clinical meaning. Regardless, it is possible that compounds that result in changes on measures used in clinical trials would have little impact on the real-world outcomes that clinicians would consider important ( 103 ), such as everyday planning and social relatedness. Measurable changes in cognition may be weakly linked to distal outcomes such as independent living or employment, particularly where such moderating factors as social networks or employment opportunities are significant. Practical clinical trials and effectiveness trials could be crucial to defining the clinical utility of cognition-enhancing medications and informing clinical decision making ( 104 ).

Several research teams are investigating the reliability and validity of clinical assessment strategies. Intermediate outcome measures, such as metacognitive scales and proxy functional measures, might be more accessible for clinicians than laboratory-based cognitive tests. Clinicians will also need better ways to assess the real-world functioning of patients with schizophrenia. More needs to be known about how clinicians should interpret patients' complaints about cognitive problems. Consumers' perspectives on cognitive interventions can be better understood, particularly regarding whether changes revealed on cognitive measures facilitate recovery and well-being ( 105 ). In summary, a range of stakeholders may provide useful perspectives on drugs in development ( 106 ) to be sure that clinical research generates the kind of data that helps prescribers direct treatments to the patients who can benefit from them.

Acknowledgments and disclosures

This work was funded by the West Los Angeles Veterans Affairs Healthcare Center and the Robert Wood Johnson Clinical Scholars Program. The views expressed are those of the author and do not necessarily reflect the views of the Robert Johnson Foundation or the Department of Veterans Affairs. The author thanks Alexander Kopelowicz, M.D, and Stephen R. Marder, M.D., for helpful comments.

The author reports no competing interests.

Dr. Bromley is affiliated with the Semel Institute Health Services Research Center, University of California, Los Angeles, and with the Veterans Affairs Desert Pacific Mental Illness Research, Education, and Clinical Center. Send correspondence to Dr. Bromley, West Los Angeles Veterans Affairs Healthcare Center, 11301 Wilshire Blvd., Building 210A (MIRECC), Room 104, Los Angeles, CA 90073 (e-mail: [email protected]).

References

1. Dickinson D, Iannone VN, Wilk CM, et al: General and specific cognitive deficits in schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry 55:826–833, 2004Google Scholar

2. Blanchard JJ, Neale JM: The neuropsychological signature of schizophrenia: generalized or differential deficit? American Journal of Psychiatry 151:40–48, 1994Google Scholar

3. Reichenberg A, Weiser M, Rapp MA, et al: Elaboration on premorbid intellectual performance in schizophrenia: premorbid intellectual decline and risk for schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry 62:1297–1304, 2005Google Scholar

4. Heinrichs RW, Zakzanis KK: Neurocognitive deficit in schizophrenia: a quantitative review of the evidence. Neuropsychology 12:426–445, 1998Google Scholar

5. Saykin AJ, Gur RC, Gur RE, et al: Neuropsychological function in schizophrenia: selective impairment in memory and learning. Archives of General Psychiatry 48:618–624, 1991Google Scholar

6. Saykin AJ, Shtasel DL, Gur RE, et al: Neuropsychological deficits in neuroleptic naive patients with first-episode schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry 51:124–131, 1994Google Scholar

7. Heaton RK, Gladsjo JA, Palmer BW, et al: Stability and course of neuropsychological deficits in schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry 58:24–32, 2001Google Scholar

8. Gold JM: Cognitive deficits as treatment targets in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research 72:21–28, 2004Google Scholar

9. Milev P, Ho BC, Arndt S, et al: Predictive values of neurocognition and negative symptoms on functional outcome in schizophrenia: a longitudinal first-episode study with 7-year follow-up. American Journal of Psychiatry 162:495–506, 2005Google Scholar

10. Green MF, Kern RS, Heaton RK: Longitudinal studies of cognition and functional outcome in schizophrenia: implications for MATRICS. Schizophrenia Research 72:41–51, 2004Google Scholar

11. Hofer A, Baumgartner S, Bodner T, et al: Patient outcomes in schizophrenia: II. the impact of cognition. European Psychiatry 20:395–402, 2005Google Scholar

12. Bowie CR, Harvey PD: Cognition in schizophrenia: impairments, determinants, and functional importance. Psychiatric Clinics of North America 28:613–633, 2005Google Scholar

13. Green MF, Nuechterlein KH: The MATRICS initiative: developing a consensus cognitive battery for clinical trials. Schizophrenia Research 72:1–3, 2004Google Scholar

14. TURNS: Treatment Units for Research on Neurocognition in Schizophrenia. Rockville, Md, National Institute of Mental Health, 2005. Available at www.turns.ucla.eduGoogle Scholar

15. Bromley E: A collaborative approach to targeted treatment development for schizophrenia: a qualitative evaluation of the NIMH-MATRICS project. Schizophrenia Bulletin 31:954–961, 2005Google Scholar

16. Nestor PJ, Scheltens P, Hodges JR: Advances in the early detection of Alzheimer's disease. Nature Medicine 10(suppl):S34–S41, 2004Google Scholar

17. Hyman SE, Fenton WS: Medicine: what are the right targets for psychopharmacology? Science 299:350–351, 2003Google Scholar

18. Zerhouni EA: US biomedical research: basic, translational, and clinical sciences. JAMA 294:1352–1358, 2005Google Scholar

19. Bartels SJ, Dums AR, Oxman TE, et al: Evidence-based practices in geriatric mental health care: an overview of systematic reviews and meta-analyses. Psychiatric Clinics of North America 26:971–990, x–xi, 2003Google Scholar

20. Lehman AF, Kreyenbuhl J, Buchanan RW, et al: The Schizophrenia Patient Outcomes Research Team (PORT): updated treatment recommendations, 2003. Schizophrenia Bulletin 30:193–217, 2004Google Scholar

21. Lehman AF, Steinwachs DM: Patterns of usual care for schizophrenia: initial results from the Schizophrenia Patient Outcomes Research Team (PORT) Client Survey. Schizophrenia Bulletin 24:11–20, 1998Google Scholar

22. Goldman HH, Azrin ST: Public policy and evidence-based practice. Psychiatric Clinics of North America 26:899–917, 2003Google Scholar

23. Nuechterlein KH, Barch DM, Gold JM, et al: Identification of separable cognitive factors in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research 72:29–39, 2004Google Scholar

24. Gorissen M, Sanz JC, Schmand B: Effort and cognition in schizophrenia patients. Schizophrenia Research 78:199–208, 2005Google Scholar

25. Koren D, Seidman LJ, Goldsmith M, et al: Real-world cognitive—and metacognitive—dysfunction in schizophrenia: a new approach for measuring (and remediating) more "right stuff." Schizophrenia Bulletin 32:310–326, 2006Google Scholar

26. Semkovska M, Bedard MA, Godbout L, et al: Assessment of executive dysfunction during activities of daily living in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research 69:289–300, 2004Google Scholar

27. Chaytor N, Schmitter-Edgecombe M: The ecological validity of neuropsychological tests: a review of the literature on everyday cognitive skills. Neuropsychology Review 13:181–197, 2003Google Scholar

28. Burgess PW, Alderman N, Evans J, et al: The ecological validity of tests of executive function. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society 4:547–558, 1998Google Scholar

29. Odhuba RA, van den Broek MD, Johns LC: Ecological validity of measures of executive functioning. British Journal of Clinical Psychology 44:269–278, 2005Google Scholar

30. Makatura TJ, Lam CS, Leahy BJ, et al: Standardized memory tests and the appraisal of everyday memory. Brain Injury 13:355–367, 1999Google Scholar

31. Hall J, Harris JM, Sprengelmeyer R, et al: Social cognition and face processing in schizophrenia. British Journal of Psychiatry 185:169–170, 2004Google Scholar

32. Calkins ME, Gur RC, Ragland JD, et al: Face recognition memory deficits and visual object memory performance in patients with schizophrenia and their relatives. American Journal of Psychiatry 162:1963–1966, 2005Google Scholar

33. Oyebode F: Cognitive processing in schizophrenia. British Journal of Psychiatry 186:262, 2005Google Scholar

34. Green MJ, Phillips ML: Social threat perception and the evolution of paranoia. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 28:333–342, 2004Google Scholar

35. Scolnick E: Program to improve cognitive functioning in patients with schizophrenia: reflections. Schizophrenia Research 72:75–77, 2004Google Scholar

36. Marder SR, Fenton W: Measurement and Treatment Research to Improve Cognition in Schizophrenia: NIMH-MATRICS initiative to support the development of agents for improving cognition in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research 72:5–9, 2004Google Scholar

37. Green MF, Nuechterlein KH, Gold JM, et al: Approaching a consensus cognitive battery for clinical trials in schizophrenia: the NIMH-MATRICS conference to select cognitive domains and test criteria. Biological Psychiatry 56:301–307, 2004Google Scholar

38. Moritz S, Ferahli S, Naber D: Memory and attention performance in psychiatric patients: lack of correspondence between clinician-rated and patient-rated functioning with neuropsychological test results. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society 10:623–633, 2004Google Scholar

39. Medalia A, Lim RW: Self-awareness of cognitive functioning in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research 71:331–338, 2004Google Scholar

40. Prouteau A, Verdoux H, Briand C, et al: Self-assessed cognitive dysfunction and objective performance in outpatients with schizophrenia participating in a rehabilitation program. Schizophrenia Research 69:85–91, 2004Google Scholar

41. Keefe RS, Poe M, Walker TM, et al: The schizophrenia cognition rating scale: an interview-based assessment and its relationship to cognition, real-world functioning, and functional capacity. American Journal of Psychiatry 163:426–432, 2006Google Scholar

42. Bauer AD, Gilmer WS, Hanlon R, et al: Subjective complaints versus objective measures of cognitive deficits in minimally ill bipolar disorder patients. Presented at the American Psychiatric Association, Toronto, Ontario, Canada, May 20–25, 2006Google Scholar

43. Burdick KE, Endick CJ, Goldberg JF: Assessing cognitive deficits in bipolar disorder: are self-reports valid? Psychiatry Research 136:43–50, 2005Google Scholar

44. Kopelman MD, Stanhope N, Guinan E: Subjective memory evaluations in patients with focal frontal, diencephalic, and temporal lobe lesion. Cortex 34:191–207, 1998Google Scholar

45. Pearman A, Storandt M: Self-discipline and self-consciousness predict subjective memory in older adults. Journal of Gerontology, Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences 60:P153–P157, 2005Google Scholar

46. Palmer BW, Heaton RK, Paulsen JS, et al: Is it possible to be schizophrenic yet neuropsychologically normal? Neuropsychology 11:437–446, 1997Google Scholar

47. Heinrichs RW, Awad AG: Neurocognitive subtypes of chronic schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research 9:49–58, 1993Google Scholar

48. Heinrichs WR: In Search of Madness: Schizophrenia and Neuroscience. Oxford, United Kingdom, Oxford University Press, 2001Google Scholar

49. Kremen WS, Seidman LJ, Faraone SV, et al: Heterogeneity of schizophrenia: a study of individual neuropsychological profiles. Schizophrenia Research 71:307–321, 2004Google Scholar

50. Weickert TW, Goldberg TE, Gold JM, et al: Cognitive impairments in patients with schizophrenia displaying preserved and compromised intellect. Archives of General Psychiatry 57:907–913, 2000Google Scholar

51. Joyce EM, Hutton SB, Mutsatsa SH, et al: Cognitive heterogeneity in first-episode schizophrenia. British Journal of Psychiatry 187:516–522, 2005Google Scholar

52. Badcock JC, Dragovic M, Waters FA, et al: Dimensions of intelligence in schizophrenia: evidence from patients with preserved, deteriorated and compromised intellect. Journal of Psychiatric Research 39:11–19, 2005Google Scholar

53. Karlsson JL: Psychosis and academic performance. British Journal of Psychiatry 184:327–329, 2004Google Scholar

54. Bilder RM, Ventura J, Cienfuegos A: Clinical Global Impression of Cognition in Schizophrenia (CGI-CogS). Interviewer's Manual and Rating Booklet, version 3.2. Los Angeles, University of California, Los Angeles, Semel Institute for Neuroscience and Human Behavior, 2003Google Scholar

55. Velligan DI, DiCocco M, Bow-Thomas CC, et al: A brief cognitive assessment for use with schizophrenia patients in community clinics. Schizophrenia Research 71:273–283, 2004Google Scholar

56. Keefe RS, Goldberg TE, Harvey PD, et al: The Brief Assessment of Cognition in Schizophrenia: reliability, sensitivity, and comparison with a standard neurocognitive battery. Schizophrenia Research 68:283–297, 2004Google Scholar

57. Gold JM, Queern C, Iannone VN, et al: Repeatable battery for the assessment of neuropsychological status as a screening test in schizophrenia: I. sensitivity, reliability, and validity. American Journal of Psychiatry 156:1944–1950, 1999Google Scholar

58. Hobart MP, Goldberg R, Bartko JJ, et al: Repeatable battery for the assessment of neuropsychological status as a screening test in schizophrenia: II. convergent/discriminant validity and diagnostic group comparisons. American Journal of Psychiatry 156:1951–1957, 1999Google Scholar

59. Zimmerman M, Posternak MA, Chelminski I, et al: Standardized Clinical Outcome Rating Scale for depression for use in clinical practice. Depression and Anxiety 22:36–40, 2005Google Scholar

60. Cole MG, McCusker J, Elie M, et al: Systematic detection and multidisciplinary care of depression in older medical inpatients: a randomized trial. Canadian Medical Association Journal 174:38–44, 2006Google Scholar

61. Lorenz KA, Ryan GW, Morton SC, et al: A qualitative examination of primary care providers' and physician managers' uses and views of research evidence. International Journal for Quality in Health Care 17:409–414, 2005Google Scholar

62. Masand PS: Breaking the barriers to optimal patient outcomes: can clinicians impact cognition? Psyche CME Reports, 2004. Available at www.psychcme.net/cme_reports.aspGoogle Scholar

63. Green MF, Barnes TR, Danion JM, et al: The FOCIS international survey on psychiatrists' opinions on cognition in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research 74:253–261, 2005Google Scholar

64. Wears RL, Berg M: Computer technology and clinical work: still waiting for Godot. JAMA 293:1261–1263, 2005Google Scholar

65. Woods DD: Designs are hypotheses about how artifacts shape cognition and collaboration. Ergonomics 41:168–173, 1998Google Scholar

66. Harvey PD, Green MF, Keefe RS, et al: Cognitive functioning in schizophrenia: a consensus statement on its role in the definition and evaluation of effective treatments for the illness. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 65:361–372, 2004Google Scholar

67. Green MF, Kern RS, Braff DL, et al: Neurocognitive deficits and functional outcome in schizophrenia: are we measuring the "right stuff"? Schizophrenia Bulletin 26:119–136, 2000Google Scholar

68. Addington J, Addington D: Neurocognitive and social functioning in schizophrenia: a 2.5 year follow-up study. Schizophrenia Research 44:47–56, 2000Google Scholar

69. Kurtz MM, Moberg PJ, Ragland JD, et al: Symptoms versus neurocognitive test performance as predictors of psychosocial status in schizophrenia: a 1- and 4-year prospective study. Schizophrenia Bulletin 31:167–174, 2005Google Scholar

70. Prouteau A, Verdoux H, Briand C, et al: Cognitive predictors of psychosocial functioning outcome in schizophrenia: a follow-up study of subjects participating in a rehabilitation program. Schizophrenia Research 77:343–353, 2005Google Scholar

71. Fujii DE, Wylie AM, Nathan JH: Neurocognition and long-term prediction of quality of life in outpatients with severe and persistent mental illness. Schizophrenia Research 69:67–73, 2004Google Scholar

72. Dickerson FB, Ringel NB, Parente F: Subjective quality of life in out-patients with schizophrenia: clinical and utilization correlates. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica 98:124–127, 1998Google Scholar

73. Brekke JS, Kohrt B, Green MF: Neuropsychological functioning as a moderator of the relationship between psychosocial functioning and the subjective experience of self and life in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin 27:697–708, 2001Google Scholar

74. Bryson G, Bell MD: Initial and final work performance in schizophrenia: cognitive and symptom predictors. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 191:87–92, 2003Google Scholar

75. Dickerson F, Boronow JJ, Ringel N, et al: Social functioning and neurocognitive deficits in outpatients with schizophrenia: a 2-year follow-up. Schizophrenia Research 37:13–20, 1999Google Scholar

76. Revheim N, Schechter I, Kim D, et al: Neurocognitive and symptom correlates of daily problem-solving skills in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research 83:237–245, 2006Google Scholar

77. Mueser KT, Salyers MP, Mueser PR: A prospective analysis of work in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin 27:281–296, 2001Google Scholar

78. McGurk SR, Mueser KT, Pascaris A: Cognitive training and supported employment for persons with severe mental illness: one-year results from a randomized controlled trial. Schizophrenia Bulletin 31:898–909, 2005Google Scholar

79. Lysaker PH, Bryson GJ, Davis LW, et al: Relationship of impaired processing speed and flexibility of abstract thought to improvements in work performance over time in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research 75:211–218, 2005Google Scholar

80. Wexler BE, Bell MD: Cognitive remediation and vocational rehabilitation for schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin 31:931–941, 2005Google Scholar

81. Evans JD, Bond GR, Meyer PS, et al: Cognitive and clinical predictors of success in vocational rehabilitation in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research 70:331–342, 2004Google Scholar

82. Krabbendam L, Aleman A: Cognitive rehabilitation in schizophrenia: a quantitative analysis of controlled studies. Psychopharmacology (Berlin) 169:376–382, 2003Google Scholar

83. Srinivasan L, Tirupati S: Relationship between cognition and work functioning among patients with schizophrenia in an urban area of India. Psychiatric Services 56:1423–1428, 2005Google Scholar

84. Srinivasan TN, Thara R: How do men with schizophrenia fare at work? A follow-up study from India. Schizophrenia Research 25:149–154, 1997Google Scholar

85. Marwaha S, Johnson S: Schizophrenia and employment: a review. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology 39:337–349, 2004Google Scholar

86. Drake RE, McHugo GJ, Bebout RR, et al: A randomized clinical trial of supported employment for inner-city patients with severe mental disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry 56:627–633, 1999Google Scholar

87. Startup M, Jackson MC, Bendix S: The concurrent validity of the Global Assessment of Functioning (GAF). British Journal of Clinical Psychology 41:417–422, 2002Google Scholar

88. Soderberg P, Tungstrom S, Armelius BA: Reliability of Global Assessment of Functioning ratings made by clinical psychiatric staff. Psychiatric Services 56:434–438, 2005Google Scholar

89. Fleischhacker WW, Rabinowitz J, Kemmler G, et al: Perceived functioning, well-being and psychiatric symptoms in patients with stable schizophrenia treated with long-acting risperidone for 1 year. British Journal of Psychiatry 187:131–136, 2005Google Scholar

90. McKibbin CL, Brekke JS, Sires D, et al: Direct assessment of functional abilities: relevance to persons with schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research 72:53–67, 2004Google Scholar

91. Patterson TL, Goldman S, McKibbin CL, et al: UCSD Performance-Based Skills Assessment: development of a new measure of everyday functioning for severely mentally ill adults. Schizophrenia Bulletin 27:235–245, 2001Google Scholar

92. Eisler RM, Hersen M, Miller PM, et al: Situational determinants of assertive behaviors. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 43:330–340, 1975Google Scholar

93. Cohen AS, Forbes CB, Mann MC, et al: Specific cognitive deficits and differential domains of social functioning impairment in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research 81:227–238, 2005Google Scholar

94. Brown CE, Rempfer MV, Hamera E, et al: Knowledge of grocery shopping skills as a mediator of cognition and performance. Psychiatric Services 57:573–575, 2006Google Scholar

95. Koren D, Seidman L, Poyurovsky M, et al: The neuropsychological basis of insight in first episode schizophrenia: a pilot metacognitive study. Schizophrenia Research 70:195–202, 2004Google Scholar

96. Karlawish JH, Lantos J: Community equipoise and the architecture of clinical research. Cambridge Quarterly of Healthcare Ethics 6:385–396, 1997Google Scholar

97. Stoppe G, Sandholzer H, Staedt J, et al: Reasons for prescribing cognition enhancers in primary care: results of a representative survey in Lower Saxony, Germany. International Journal of Clinical Pharmacology and Therapeutics 33:486–490, 1995Google Scholar

98. Livingston G, Katona C: The place of memantine in the treatment of Alzheimer's disease: a number needed to treat analysis. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 19:919–925, 2004Google Scholar

99. Doody R: Refining treatment guidelines in Alzheimer's disease. Geriatrics Supplement:14–20, 2005Google Scholar

100. Benbow S, Jones R, Jolley D: Prescribing: short rations. Health Services Journal 109:26–27, 1999Google Scholar

101. Healy D: Shaping the intimate: influences on the experience of everyday nerves. Social Studies of Science 34:219–245, 2004Google Scholar

102. Karlawish JH, Clark CM: Addressing the challenges of transforming laboratory advances into Alzheimer's disease treatments. Neurobiology of Aging 23:1043–1049, 2002Google Scholar

103. Harvey PD: Conference report: the Seventh Biennial Mt Sinai Conference on Cognition in Schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin 31:895–897, 2005Google Scholar

104. Tunis SR, Stryer DB, Clancy CM: Practical clinical trials: increasing the value of clinical research for decision making in clinical and health policy. JAMA 290:1624–1632, 2003Google Scholar

105. Essock SM, Goldman HH, Van Tosh L, et al: Evidence-based practices: setting the context and responding to concerns. Psychiatric Clinics of North America 26:919–938, ix, 2003Google Scholar

106. Tunis S, Korn A, Ommaya A: The Role of Purchasers and Payers in the Clinical Research Enterprise. Washington, DC, Institute of Medicine and National Academies Press, 2002Google Scholar