The American Psychiatric Association (APA) has updated its Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including with new information specifically addressed to individuals in the European Economic Area. As described in the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, this website utilizes cookies, including for the purpose of offering an optimal online experience and services tailored to your preferences.

Please read the entire Privacy Policy and Terms of Use. By closing this message, browsing this website, continuing the navigation, or otherwise continuing to use the APA's websites, you confirm that you understand and accept the terms of the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including the utilization of cookies.

×

Abstract

Objective

Public stigma and discrimination have pernicious effects on the lives of people with serious mental illnesses. Given a plethora of research on changing the stigma of mental illness, this article reports on a meta-analysis that examined the effects of antistigma approaches that included protest or social activism, education of the public, and contact with persons with mental illness.

Methods

The investigators heeded published guidelines for systematic literature reviews in health care. This comprehensive and systematic review included articles in languages other than English, dissertations, and population studies. The search included all articles from the inception of the databases until October 2010. Search terms fell into three categories: stigma, mental illness (such as schizophrenia and depression), and change program (including contact and education). The search yielded 72 articles and reports meeting the inclusion criteria of relevance to changing public stigma and sufficient data and statistics to complete analyses. Studies represented 38,364 research participants from 14 countries. Effect sizes were computed for all studies and for each treatment condition within studies. Comparisons between effect sizes were conducted with a weighted one-way analysis of variance.

Results

Overall, both education and contact had positive effects on reducing stigma for adults and adolescents with a mental illness. However, contact was better than education at reducing stigma for adults. For adolescents, the opposite pattern was found: education was more effective. Overall, face-to-face contact was more effective than contact by video.

Conclusions

Future research is needed to identify moderators of the effects of both education and contact.

Stigma has broadly harmful effects on the lives of people with mental illness. Public stigma (the prejudice and discrimination endorsed by the general population that affects a person) has been distinguished from self-stigma (the harm that occurs when the person internalizes the prejudice) with attempts to craft separate interventions for each type of stigma (1,2). Advocates from around the world have been trying to erase the public stigma of mental illness for more than 50 years (3). In the 1950s, Cummings and Cummings (4) used group discussions and films throughout a small Canadian town to affect mental illness stigma. Since 1996, the World Psychiatric Association has promoted its Open-the-Doors programs, which has produced a slew of evaluations (3,5). Australia’s beyondblue initiative to raise awareness and reduce stigma surrounding depression has been in place for more than a decade, whereas Canada and the United Kingdom have jumped into the antistigma fray only in the past couple of years.

Background

The United States first systematically pursued antistigma campaigns after the 1999 White House Conference on Mental Health. American efforts against stigma have soared given the energy and resources of professional groups (the American Psychiatric Association and American Psychological Association), advocacy groups (such as the National Alliance on Mental Illness [NAMI] and Mental Health America), pharmaceutical companies (Eli Lilly), and government bodies (the Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration and the National Institute of Mental Health). Research has begun to examine the effect of various programs intended to erase public stigma. This article reports on a meta-analysis of findings reported in the available research literature.

Three approaches to change

Approaches to changing public stigma have been divided into three paradigms on the basis of a review of social- psychological research related to racial-ethnic and gender minority groups: education, contact, and protest (6). Educational approaches to stigma challenge inaccurate stereotypes about mental illnesses, replacing them with factual information (for example, contrary to the myth that people with mental illnesses are homicidal maniacs, the difference in the rate of homicides by people with serious psychiatric disorders versus the general public is very small). Educational strategies have included public service announcements, books, flyers, movies, videos, Web pages, podcasts, virtual reality, and other audiovisual aids (7,8). Some benefits of educational interventions include their low cost and broad reach.

A second strategy for reducing stigma is interpersonal contact with members of the stigmatized group. Individuals of the general population who meet and interact with people with mental illnesses are likely to lessen their levels of prejudice (9). Social-psychological research has identified factors that seem to moderate contact effects (10,11), including one-to-one contact so that people who engage with one another can learn of similar interests and potentially cultivate a friendship (12,13), contact that includes a common goal (14), and interactions with a person who moderately disconfirms prevailing stereotypes (15,16).

Social activism, or protest, is the third form of stigma change we examined. Protest strategies highlight the injustices of various forms of stigma and chastise offenders for their stereotypes and discrimination: “Shame on us all for perpetuating the ideas that people with mental illness are just ‘big kids’ unable to care for themselves.” There is anecdotal evidence suggesting that protest can reduce harmful media representations (17). However, some research implies protest campaigns that ask people to suppress prejudice can produce an unintended “rebound” in which prejudices about a group remain unchanged or actually become worse (1820). In one set of studies, Macrae and colleagues (18) found that research participants directed to suppress stereotypes about skinheads showed greater stereotype activation and increased distance from members of that group.

Outcomes of public stigma change have been assessed in several ways, with self-reported social-cognitive measures dominating (2123). These are sometimes understood in terms of a simple path model: attitudes, affect, and behavior (9). Attitudes reflect the stereotypes about mental illness and include ideas about blame, dangerousness, and incompetence. Affect concerns the emotional reaction to attitudes: dangerousness begets fear, and blame yields to anger. Discriminatory behavior results from stereotypic attitudes and affect; for example, the public is hesitant to employ (2428) or to rent property (2831) to people with mental illness. Behavior per se is a difficult construct to assess because most studies do not have resources to observe actual responses after stigmatizing attitudes and affect. This kind of assessment requires some form of independent observation in an overwhelming number of settings and times. As a result, many studies fail to assess change in discriminatory actions. More likely, self-report measures used as outcome indicators in public stigma change studies represent behavioral intentions—the self-reported likelihood that people somehow anticipate distancing themselves or otherwise avoiding a person with mental illness (22).

Past reviews

Two reviews have attempted to make sense of the body of research on public stigma change. Holzinger and colleagues (32) summarized 51 studies of interventions targeting public stigma conducted in real-world settings with members of the general public. They did not include investigations using experimental or other laboratory designs. Documents were located through review of PubMed, gray literature, and the Internet; psychological databases (PsycNET) and replication studies were excluded. They concluded that education and contact conditions seem to have positive effects on attitudes. Their findings on the impact on behavior were less clear. Outcomes in this review were coded solely as positive or negative, with no effect sizes, so inferential statistics were not used to more carefully make sense of education versus contact effects. Moreover, no careful coding schema was used to summarize treatment methods or outcome variables. The list of studies also did not include several prominent investigations in the field. Another older review focused solely on contact effects (33). The review considered 22 studies, dividing them into retrospective (reporting the effects of previous contact) and prospective (the effects of planned contact where an advocate interacts with a group). The authors concluded that both types had robust inverse effects on stigma. Like the review by Holzinger and colleagues, however, the Couture and Penn study (33) did not determine effect sizes or provide any careful codes of research design. Moreover, retrospective studies would not fall under the rubric of stigma change as examined here—that is, a planful effort to change stereotypes, prejudice, and discrimination of targeted groups.

Given this background, we pursued a meta-analysis of strategies to change public stigma, keeping several aims in mind. We sought to describe the degree to which the social-cognitive model of stigma (attitudes, affect, and behavior intention) has been tested. Consistent with research on the prejudice related to ethnicity and gender, we hypothesized that contact strategies would yield significantly better effects than education (6). We describe the quality of research designs used to examine these questions. Two-by-two (pre-post) randomized controlled trials offer the most rigorous test of contact versus education; hence, effect sizes from just those studies were separately examined. We examined the effects of milieu for establishing contact—via video or in person—and hypothesized that the grassroots nature of meeting in person would lead to better effects. We also tested how antistigma programs influence the stigmatizing attitudes and behavioral intentions of adolescents, an important target group.

Methods

Selection

We used the Centre for Reviews and Dissemination’s guidelines for systematic reviews in health care (34) to organize and conduct the meta-analysis. The guidelines include a comprehensive algorithm for locating relevant studies in the broadest sphere of published documents, coding the studies in a comprehensive and reliable manner, and determining valid effect sizes that serve as the foundation for analyses of important research goals. We adopted a snowball strategy for finding studies. We began with databases most likely to yield peer-reviewed or mentor-reviewed papers: PsycNET, PubMed, Scopus, and Dissertation Abstracts. Searches yielded primary sources, and we then reviewed the reference sections of those sources to identify additional candidates for consideration. Searches focused on articles from the inception of the database up to October 2010. We also made a special effort to identify articles in languages other than English. Several articles identified in the above searches were reviewed by colleagues bilingual in Chinese, German, French, Polish, and Spanish. In addition, we searched several Indian databases (Scientific Journal Publishing in India, HRH Global Resource Center Web page, connectjournals.com, Indian Academy of Sciences, Indian Institute of Science, Indian National Science Academy, and Indianjournals.com) and Chinese databases (the China National Knowledge Infrastructure and Chinese Electronic Periodical Services). Despite this effort, only two German language articles were found to meet inclusion criteria. Finally, we cross-walked our collection of articles with the recent review by Holzinger and colleagues (32).

We used three sets of search terms: stigma, mental illness, and change program. Mental illness was searched using the broad rubric plus more specific terms like “mental disorder” or individual diagnoses. Along with “change program,” seven additional search terms were used that represented specific programs (education or contact) or more general synonyms (stigma reduction or antistigma program). Using the search terms, we located more than 8,700 documents, and 72 of them ultimately provided useable data to address our goals. The review and winnowing process is summarized in Figure 1. Note that most articles yielded multiple effect sizes, depending on condition, trials, and outcome variables therein.

Figure 1 A summary of the review process guiding the meta-analysis

Coding

A code book was developed to summarize each study; codes represented key concepts about public stigma change described in our introduction. Specific codes included descriptors of research participants, research design, type of stigma change program, outcome measures, and moderating variables. Type of antistigma intervention was indicated by one of three codes: protest, contact, and education. Clear definitions of interventions were provided to ensure that studies were grouped appropriately (these definitions and the code book are available from the first author).

We coded for type of outcome variable in terms of attitude, affect, or behavioral intention. We entered number of research participants assigned to each group and target of the intervention. Some studies looked at developmental issues; therefore, we coded for whether studies included children and adolescents. Some studies looked at effects on power groups, including employers, health care professionals, and police officers. Demographic characteristics of targets were also entered, including gender, age, race-ethnicity, and education level. Two graduate student raters were trained on the coding schema and had to meet the reliability criterion on five articles (κ=.84) before working independently. After review of more than 30 articles, interrater reliability was checked again for drift but remained high (κ=.94).

Effect size analysis

The effect size used in this study was the standardized mean difference (35) comparing the means of persons who received the intervention versus a contrast condition, divided by the within-group standard deviation of scores. Effect sizes were scaled so that that a positive effect size would indicate less stigmatizing attitudes among those who received the intervention. Data were obtained from a variety of research designs, so effect sizes were computed with all available information from each design (36,37). Effect sizes were computed from means and standard deviations where available. In other cases, effect size was computed from available statistics (t or F) or p values with the use of formulas appropriate to each research design (38).

A few studies included more than one treatment condition. Wherever conditions represented different types of intervention, a separate effect size was computed for each intervention that met the inclusion criteria. Otherwise, effect sizes were averaged to produce a single effect size for the type of intervention. Many studies included multiple outcome measures. Variables representing distinct outcome constructs were maintained as separate effect sizes. When multiple measures of the same construct were provided, these were included as separate effect sizes for subsequent analyses. Comparisons between effect sizes were conducted with a weighted one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) from the SPSS macro MetaF (39).

Results

We found 72 articles that were related to changing the public stigma of mental illness with data suitable to the meta-analyses described herein (7,40109). Table 1 provides summary statistics of these studies, including mean frequencies of demographic characteristics determined by summing frequencies from each study that provided a specific demographic and dividing by total number of studies. We also determined mean and standard deviation of participants’ average ages in a similar manner. The 72 articles represented 79 independent studies with 38,364 research participants from 14 countries; by continent, they were from Europe (N=22,179), North America (N=14,307), South America (N=63), Asia (N=1,299), and Australia (N=516). There were none from Africa. The median number of research participants per study was 150.0 (mean±SD=540.3±1,279.3). Articles were published from January 1972 to October 2010. The literature search yielded 612 effect sizes. Of the research participants, 58.7% were female, and most were European or European American (>60%). The participants were mostly single (>70%) and students (≥60%) with a college education or higher (>60%).

Table 1 Key descriptors in 79 studies of countering public stigma
CharacteristicFrequency (%)Range (%)
Research participants
 Age (M±SD)27.7±10.415–49
 Female58.7±18.43–100
 Ethnicity
  European or European American61.1±27.10–95
  African or African American21.1±29.80–100
  Asian or Asian American6.1±12.90–52
  Hispanic or Hispanic American5.6±9.40–37
  Other7.8±12.50–52
 Marital status
  Married22.1±25.90–66
  Single70.8±33.718–100
  Divorced3.5±3.60–9
  Widowed3.6±8.60–23
 Employment status
  Full-time27.8±42.80–100
  Part-time1.7±8.30–47
  Student69.0±45.20–100
  Unemployed1.9±5.80–29
 Educational attainment
  Less than high school31.6±47.00–100
  High school diploma or GED5.8±13.60–59
  Some college46.0±46.90–100
  4-year degree11.8±24.00–100
  Graduate degree5.2±15.00–67
 Target of stigma change
  College students27.2
  Children under 12.8
  Adolescents24.1
  Adults (>18)26.5
  Professionals4.7
  Family members.3
  Students in professional programs8.5
  Criminal justice professionals7.2
Quality of antistigma program
 Antistigma manual (% yes)40.1
 Manual training (% yes)14.2
Outcome measurea
 Attitudesb
  Competence4.2
  Dangerousness6.5
  Empowerment1.0
  Poor prognosis4.2
  Responsibility4.2
  Benevolence2.1
  Negative perceptions1.0
  Personality.3
  Credibility.3
 Affectc
  Anger1.6
  Fear2.8
  Pity1.5
  Shame.8
 Behavioral intentionsd
  Avoidance10.9
  Coercion2.4
  Help2.4
  Segregation2.9
  Authoritarian2.1
  Community health ideology1.6
  Social restrictiveness2.4
  Interpersonal ideology.8

a Values indicate percentage of studies that used a measure of attitude, affect, or behavioral intentions.

b Test-retest reliability (N=44)=.730±.160; Cronbach’s alpha (N=37)=.760±.150

c Test-retest reliability (N=0) was not applicable; Cronbach’s alpha (N=6)=.835±.090

d Test-retest reliability (N=32)=.755±.070; Cronbach’s alpha (N=29)=.811±.080

Table 1 Key descriptors in 79 studies of countering public stigma
Enlarge table

An important goal of antistigma programs is to strategically target groups who, by virtue of age or role, have importance in terms of the broad stigma change picture (110). For example, employers who endorse stigma may be less likely to hire people with mental illness and therefore might be important targets. Table 1 provides the frequency with which the 79 studies targeted specific groups. Although seemingly an important group, children under age 12 were rarely the focus of stigma change (.8%); adolescents, on the other hand, were far more common targets, with almost 25% of studies we examined targeting stigma among teenagers. Professionals (4.7%) and students in professional programs (8.5%) were relatively more common targets of stigma change, as were criminal justice professionals (7.2%). This information on targeted groups was the only proxy coded for setting in which a specific antistigma program was conducted.

One way we sought to index the quality of the antistigma programs was whether there was evidence of some sort of manual or training guiding the program. About 40% of studies reported use of an antistigma program manual. About 14% acknowledged some sort of training of program facilitators. We also coded for studies that checked fidelity of the antistigma program and found none.

An important finding of meta-analyses is an audit of the type of outcome measures used to assess effects. The bottom of Table 1 provides frequencies with which constructs were examined in individual studies. They are divided into the three outcome areas: attitudes, affect, and behavioral intentions. Frequent measures of attitudes included dangerousness, competence, responsibility, and poor prognosis. Affect was much less often assessed and was represented by proxies of fear and anger. Behavioral intentions were most often represented by measures of avoidance. We coded psychometric values of assessments where available; summaries are provided in Table 1 by outcome type (see table footnotes). Test-retest reliabilities were not found for measures of affect; six alphas were obtained for this domain, with the mean being high (>.8). Thirty or more test-retest reliabilities or alphas were provided for measures of attitudes and behavioral intentions; all of their means were satisfactory, exceeding .725. The next section summarizes effect sizes by antistigma approach. Each analysis provides both an overall result (across all outcomes), as well as separate results for each type of outcome measure.

Effects of contact and education on public stigma change

The means and standard errors of effect sizes (d) are organized in Table 2 by antistigma approach; the table also includes the number of effect sizes gleaned from the literature per approach (K). Note that this number was often above the 72 articles and reports and 79 individual studies found from the review because most investigations used multiple dependent measures or comparisons to test for differences. [A forest plot summarizing the effect sizes from the overall analysis is provided online as a data supplement to this article. The graph depicts the effect size estimate from each study, along with the 95% confidence intervals (111).]

Table 2 Overall effect of antistigma programs and effects on attitudes, affect, and behavioral intentions in 79 studiesa
Antistigma approachOverallb
Attitudesc
Affectd
Behavioral intentionse
dSESDKQwdSESDKQwdSESDKQwdSESDKQw
All studies
 Protest.099.284.0964.006000
 Education.286***.023.304431335.5.310***.026.291280209.8.144*.067.2612427.2.251***.048.363127106.7
 Contact.282***.036.583177261.1***.406***.046.61193156.2***–.030.077.1971714.4.189**.068.6166680.3
Randomized controlled trials (N=13)
 Education.153**.034.15811628.8.207*.054.1185019.1.103*.046.1705533.8
 Contact.363***.069.44371145.8***.626***.146.6312769.9***.268**.060.3583356.6**

a d, adjusted mean effect size; SE, standard error of the mean effect size; SD, standard deviation of the mean effect size; K, number of effect sizes; Qw, homogeneity

b Between-groups comparisons: all studies, Q=18.37, df=2 and 611, p<.001; randomized controlled trials (RCTs), Q=4.80, df=1 and 186, p<.05

c Between-groups comparisons: all studies, Q=6.17, df=1 and 372, p<.05; RCTs, Q=4.38, df=1 and 76, p<.05

d Between-groups comparisons: all studies, Q=2.87, df=1 and 40, p=.20; RCTs, not applicable

e Between-groups comparisons: all studies, Q=4.80, df=1 and 192, p=.73; RCTs, Q=4.70, df=1 and 87, p<.05

*p<.05

**p<.01

***p<.001

Table 2 Overall effect of antistigma programs and effects on attitudes, affect, and behavioral intentions in 79 studiesa
Enlarge table

Two kinds of significant differences were examined in this meta-analysis. The first was whether the effect size differed significantly from zero effect. Mean d, representing the overall effect for protest, was .099, which did not differ significantly from zero effect, suggesting that protest led to no significant change in outcome. Note that Cohen (112) defined effect sizes as negligible (<.10), small (.10–.30), medium (.30–.50), and large (>.50). Only four effect sizes representing protest effects (K=4) emerged from the literature, and they were all obtained from only one study. Hence, for the remainder of the analyses, we considered only the pattern of effect sizes for education versus contact.

Table 2 shows overall effect sizes for contact and education each significantly differed from zero (p<.001) and were relatively small (between .10 and .30). This was based on 431 and 177 d values for education and contact, respectively. The second kind of significant difference examined in this study was between the distribution of effect sizes for antistigma groups. Results of a random-effects, one-way ANOVA showed mean effect sizes differed significantly between protest, education, and contact approaches to changing public stigma. However, an additional random-effects ANOVA failed to show that effect sizes between contact and education differed significantly. Mean d also differed significantly from zero for both approaches when change in attitudes was assessed and yielded medium effect sizes. Table 2 includes the weighted one-way ANOVA for difference in these d values (total K=373) and showed a significant difference (Q=6.17, p<.05) for attitude effects, with contact with persons with mental illness leading to greater improvement in changing public stigma. Education was shown to yield significant improvements in the affect and behavioral intention domains as well, with small effect sizes. Contact yielded significant improvements in behavioral intentions but not in affect. Mean d values did not differ significantly between education and contact across affect and behavioral intentions.

Variability of design quality across studies is a concern in meta-analyses, because effect sizes arise out of research studies that differ in experimental rigor. Meta-analysts have written much about indices of design quality and seem to agree that studies using randomized controlled trials (RCTs) require similar procedural manipulations to make them relatively similar and methodologically strong (113). Thirteen articles reported RCTs, yielding 187 effect sizes. These values are summarized in the bottom of Table 2. Once again, the mean overall d for education and contact was significantly different from zero effect (p<.01 and .001, respectively). Moreover, results of the weighted one-way ANOVA were significant, with the mean effect size for contact being significantly greater than for education (p<.05). Table 2 also includes effect sizes by attitude and behavioral intention. Education and contact yielded effect sizes significantly greater than zero for both constructs. Weighted one-way ANOVAs were significant for attitudes and behavioral intentions (p<.05), with mean effect size for contact larger than that for education in both instances.

Video versus in-person effects

Table 2 includes Qw as an index of homogeneity, or the degree to which variance across studies within a subgroup is larger than expected due to chance. Significant Qw values were found for half the distribution of effect sizes for contact approaches (top of Table 2), and for three out of four of the d distributions for the RCTs. These findings suggest that moderators may further explain some of the significance of the contact effect. Our review of the literature suggested one variable that might moderate effects: the means of contact (contact in person versus via video). Namely, was contact from someone with mental illness provided in person or on videotape? Table 3 summarizes mean d values for overall effect size as well as for attitudes and behavioral intentions across studies that used contact in person versus by video.

Table 3 Effectiveness of in-person versus video contact with a person with mental illness and effectiveness of education versus contact in antistigma programs for adolescentsa
Antistigma approachOverallb
Attitudesc
Behavioral intentionsd
Mean dSESDKQwMean dSESDKQwMean dSESDKQw
All studies (N=79)
  Contact in person.516**.060.4735881.6*.656***.086.5133434.5.397***.075.4622216.3
  Contact by video.155*.048.2118354.2.296*.122.0874460.3*.197**.063.1772626.7
Studies focusing on adolescents (N=19)
 Education.392***.056.2627898.7*.453***.065.3645050.1.302*.133.2142233.4*
 Contact.244***.061.1776846.1.242**.068.2134643.0.303*.150.281185.0
  In person.401***.090.3382312.7.371**.120.377158.3.457**.131.34283.3
  By video.166**.060.1554551.5.183**.080.1433134.9.172.098.1211011.7

a d, adjusted mean effect size; SE, standard error of the mean effect size; SD, standard deviation of the mean effect size; K, number of effect sizes; Qw, homogeneity

b Between-groups comparisons: contact approach (all studies), Q=22.10, df=1 and 140, p<.001; education versus contact (studies of adolescents), Q=3.21, df=1 and 145, p<.10; contact approach (studies of adolescents), Q=4.73, df=1 and 67, p<.05

c Between-groups comparisons: contact approach (all studies), Q=18.70, df=1 and 77, p<.001; education versus contact (studies of adolescents), Q=4.98, df=1 and 95, p<.05; contact approach (studies of adolescents), Q=1.70, df=1 and 45, p=.33

d Between-groups comparisons: contact approach (all studies), Q=4.24, df=1 and 47, p<.05; education versus contact (studies of adolescents), Q=.00, df=1 and 39, p=1.00; contact approach (studies of adolescents), Q=3.05, df=1 and 17, p<.10

*p<.05

**p<.01

***p<.001

Table 3 Effectiveness of in-person versus video contact with a person with mental illness and effectiveness of education versus contact in antistigma programs for adolescentsa
Enlarge table

The mean d for overall effect size was significant for both types of contact. A weighted one-way ANOVA showed that compared with contact by video, in-person contact had a significantly greater effect size (p<.001). Moreover, effect sizes for the two types of contact were significantly greater than zero for attitudes and behavioral intention. One-way weighted ANOVAs showed in both cases that effect size after in-person contact was significantly greater than that yielded by videotaped contact.

Effects on adolescents

Some advocates believe that antistigma programs for children are an effective way to prevent stigma from emerging in adulthood; although very few studies targeted children under age 12, almost a quarter (N=19) targeted adolescents. Table 3 summarizes effect sizes across education and contact conditions for this age group. Findings were a bit different from those reported for overall samples. Mean effect sizes showed education and contact both led to significantly increased overall effects as well as to change in attitudes and behaviors. However, unlike the ANOVA for adults, the one-way weighted ANOVA examining mean effect sizes for attitudes showed education yielded a larger effect than contact. No significant differences were found in mean effect sizes for education and contact with overall effect size or behavioral intention.

Table 3 also shows results of in-person and video contact with adolescents; these findings mostly paralleled those of the total meta-analysis in the upper portion of Table 3. Effects by contact type led to significant changes in overall outcome. Moreover, the one-way weighted ANOVA showed that in-person contact yielded greater overall effects than contact by video. In-person contact yielded significant change in attitudes and behavioral intentions, whereas video contact showed significant improvements only in attitudes. A one-way ANOVA for difference in effect sizes for behavioral intentions yielded nonsignificant trends (p<.10), suggesting in-person contact had a bigger effect than video contact for adolescents.

Discussion

Although contact and education both seem to significantly improve attitudes and behavioral intentions toward people with mental illness, contact seems to yield significantly better change, at least among adults. This is especially evident in studies that used more rigorous research designs, such as RCTs. Mean effect sizes for contact when assessing overall effects as well as effects on attitudes and behavioral intentions were significantly greater than those found for education. Meeting people with serious mental illness seems to do more to challenge stigma than educationally contrasting myths versus facts of mental illness.

One additional finding—and the most important—emerged from the meta-analysis: face-to-face contact with the person, and not a story mediated by videotape, had the greatest effect. This was evident for overall impact as well as for changing attitudes and behavioral intentions. This finding juxtaposes two important agendas in stigma change: broad audience versus grassroots control. Videotaped contact has the potential for a broad audience: disseminating the video via a variety of online platforms and television networks exponentially increases exposure of the antistigma effort compared with face-to-face approaches. However, the social marketing campaigns that rely on videotaped contact diminish grassroots control of the effort. Social marketing campaigns are often managed by government and advertising consortia rather than by people with mental illness who are targeting key groups at the local level for meaningful stigma change. In-person contact leads to better effects, but both types of contact significantly diminish stigma. Those crafting an antistigma campaign need to balance relative effect sizes with population served and grassroots control issues.

Additional intriguing differences were found when we focused on contact and education effects on adolescents. Once again, both education and contact were shown to significantly affect stigma overall, as well as attitudes and behavioral intentions. However, contrary to the other findings across all studies, education yielded significantly greater effects on attitudes than contact did. Perhaps this difference emerged because adolescents’ beliefs about mental illness are not as firmly developed as adults’ and adolescents therefore are more likely to be responsive to education effects. Compared with adults, adolescents show more variance in response to stigma measures and hence have more room for change. What implications do these findings have for ongoing campaigns for children and adolescents? They may bolster the need for caution, as many have voiced, about using peers as contacts—that is, high school students telling their stories about mental illness and recovery. Advocates have been concerned about risks for persons with mental illness in this age group compared with risks for adults in similar circumstances. Still, it is unclear from the meta-analysis whether the relatively muted effects of contact compared with education occurred because the person providing contact to the adolescents was close to their age. Generally, similarity between contact and audience is needed for most credibility and greatest antistigma effects (110). Cohort and developmental differences need to be considered in future research on contact programs in schools. It is also important to note that in-person contact seemed to produce greater effects than video-based attempts.

We found that protest or social activism concerning public stigma was rarely examined in research. In part, investigators seem to be “voting with their feet,” suggesting that protest is not a good strategy to affect stigmatizing attitudes and behavioral intentions. In fact, the effect sizes found in the meta-analysis did not show that protest yielded significant changes in stigma. Still, protest probably should not be discarded entirely. Although it is not an effective avenue for changing attitudes, it may be useful in suppressing behaviors that promote stigma, especially in the media. Programs like NAMI’s StigmaBusters have targeted stigmatizing advertisements, news stories, and entertainment through strategic letter-writing campaigns. Anecdotally, these seem to have had some effects—for example, one campaign led the American Broadcasting Company to pull its television drama Wonderland in 2000 after two episodes. The show stoked stereotypic connections between mental illness and violence. Research is needed to determine whether anecdotes like these translate to meaningful impact on stigma in the media.

Meta-analyses are known for their problems, and this one had its share. The 22 outcome measures summarized in Table 2 were reduced to three effect sizes, thereby losing theoretical and methodological sensitivity. Contact and education are complex processes, yet each was squeezed into a single mold for our analyses. We were unable to report separate analyses on follow-up effects because of the significant differences in their definitions in the relatively few studies that reported them. Despite the call for targeted antistigma programs, such audiences were not strategically sought in the studies we found on stigma change. Another way to target stigma change is by diagnostic group: messages that frame the stigma of schizophrenia versus the stigma of major depression. Programs with this kind of distinction were largely not present in the literature. Outcome was largely limited to self-report. Given that discriminatory behaviors are perhaps the most egregious of stigma’s impacts, examination of change in behavior was regrettably missing.

Conclusions

One of the benefits of reviews like these, and the methodological audits on which they rest, is the directions signaled for future research. Both education and contact have some value in stigma change. Future research needs to identify moderators of these effects. They include adjustments within the intervention (for example, the means of contact) as well as to the target of the intervention. Degree of exposure to the program is an important parameter. Greater resources are needed to go beyond psychological self-reports to observe actual behavior change.

One area not discussed in this review is the impact of multiple stigmas and how antistigma programs should be crafted to address the interaction of multiple prejudices—for example, mental illness and demographic characteristics (such as race, gender, or sexual orientation) or other health conditions (such as HIV/AIDS or physical disabilities). This concern brings us back to the grassroots goal, namely that stigma is a local issue shaped by the experience of mental illness in a variety of social contexts. Hence, stigma change, and evaluation of this change, must be conducted at the local level. This calls for future research that is dominated by community-based participatory research and investigations marked by partnerships between those skilled in research methods and local advocates.

With the exception of Dr. Rüsch, the authors are affiliated with the Illinois Institute of Technology, 3424 S. State St., Chicago, IL 60616 (e-mail: ).
Dr. Rüsch is with the Department of General and Social Psychiatry, Psychiatric University Hospital Zürich, Switzerland.

Acknowledgments and disclosures

The authors report no competing interests.

References

1 Hinshaw S: The Mark of Shame: Stigma of Mental Illness and an Agenda for Change. New York, Oxford University Press, 2007Google Scholar

2 Thornicroft G: Shunned: Discrimination Against People With Mental Illness. New York, Oxford University Press, 2006Google Scholar

3 Arboleda-Flórez J, Sartorius N: Understanding the Stigma of Mental Illness: Theory and Interventions. Chichester, England, Wiley, 2008CrossrefGoogle Scholar

4 Cummings E, Cummings J: Closed Ranks: An Experiment in Mental Health. Cambridge, Mass, Harvard University Press, 1957CrossrefGoogle Scholar

5 Sartorius N, Schulze H: Reducing the Stigma of Mental Illness: A Report From a Global Programme of the World Psychiatric Association. Cambridge, United Kingdom, Cambridge University Press, 2005CrossrefGoogle Scholar

6 Corrigan PW, Penn DL: Lessons from social psychology on discrediting psychiatric stigma. American Psychologist 54:765–776, 1999Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

7 Finkelstein J, Lapshin O, Wasserman E: Randomized study of different anti-stigma media. Patient Education and Counseling 71:204–214, 2008Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

8 Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration (SAMHSA): Mental Illness: What a Difference a Friend Makes. www.whatadifference.samhsa.gov. Accessed Aug 12, 2011Google Scholar

9 Corrigan P: On the Stigma of Mental Illness: Practical Strategies for Research and Social Change. Washington, DC, American Psychological Association, 2005CrossrefGoogle Scholar

10 Allport G: The Nature of Prejudice. Oxford, England, Addison-Wesley, 1954Google Scholar

11 Pettigrew T, Tropp L: Does intergroup contact reduce prejudice: recent meta-analytic findings; in Reducing Prejudice and Discrimination. Edited by Oskamp S. Mahwah, NJ, Erlbaum, 2000Google Scholar

12 Herek GM, Capitanio JP: “Some of my best friends” intergroup contact, concealable stigma, and heterosexuals’ attitudes toward gay men and lesbians. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 22:412–424, 1996CrossrefGoogle Scholar

13 Levin S, van Laar C, Sidanius J: The effects of ingroup and outgroup friendships on ethnic attitudes in college: a longitudinal study. Group Processes and Intergroup Relations 6:76–92, 2003CrossrefGoogle Scholar

14 Cook SW: Experimenting on social issues: the case of school desegregation. American Psychologist 40:452–460, 1985Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

15 Blanchard F, Weigel RH, Cook SW: The effect of relative competence of group members upon interpersonal attraction in cooperating interracial groups. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 32:519–530, 1975CrossrefGoogle Scholar

16 Reinke RR, Corrigan PW, Leonhard C, et al.: Examining two aspects of contact on the stigma of mental illness. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology 23:377–389, 2004CrossrefGoogle Scholar

17 Wahl O: Media Madness: Public Images of Mental Illness. Piscataway, NJ, Rutgers University Press, 1995Google Scholar

18 Macrae C, Bodenhausen GV, Milne AB, et al.: Out of mind but back in sight: stereotypes on the rebound. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 67:808–817, 1994CrossrefGoogle Scholar

19 Wegner DM, Erber R, Zanakos S: Ironic processes in the mental control of mood and mood-related thought. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 65:1093–1104, 1993Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

20 Wegner DM, Schneider DJ: Mental control: the war of the ghosts in the machine; in Unintended Thought. Edited by Uleman JBargh J. New York, Guilford, 1989Google Scholar

21 Brohan E, Slade M, Clement S, et al.: Experiences of mental illness stigma, prejudice and discrimination: a review of measures. BMC Health Services Research 10:80, 2010Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

22 Corrigan PW, Shapiro JR: Measuring the impact of programs that challenge the public stigma of mental illness. Clinical Psychology Review 30:907–922, 2010Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

23 Link BG, Yang LH, Phelan JC, et al.: Measuring mental illness stigma. Schizophrenia Bulletin 30:511–541, 2004Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

24 Bordieri JE, Drehmer DE: Hiring decisions for disabled workers: looking at the cause. Journal of Applied Social Psychology 16:197–208, 1986CrossrefGoogle Scholar

25 Farina A, Feliner RD: Employment interviewer reactions to former mental patients. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 82:268–272, 1973Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

26 Farina A, Felner RD, Boudreau LA: Reactions of workers to male and female mental patient job applicants. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 41:363–372, 1973Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

27 Link B: Understanding labeling effects in the area of mental disorders: an assessment of the effects of expectations of rejection. American Sociological Review 52:96–112, 1987CrossrefGoogle Scholar

28 Wahl OF: Mental health consumers’ experience of stigma. Schizophrenia Bulletin 25:467–478, 1999Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

29 Aviram U, Segal SP: Exclusion of the mentally ill: reflection on an old problem in a new context. Archives of General Psychiatry 29:126–131, 1973Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

30 Farina A, Thaw J, Lovern JD, et al.: People’s reactions to a former mental patient moving to their neighborhood. Journal of Community Psychology 2:108–112, 1974CrossrefGoogle Scholar

31 Page S: Oversimplified images: comments on “Control or Treatment? Experiences of People Who Have Been Psychiatrically Hospitalized in New Brunswick”. Canadian Journal of Community Mental Health 14:129–132, 1995CrossrefGoogle Scholar

32 Holzinger A, Dietrich S, Heitmann S, et al.: Evaluation of target-group oriented interventions aimed at reducing the stigma surrounding mental illness [in German]. Psychiatrische Praxis 35:376–386, 2008Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

33 Couture S, Penn D: Interpersonal contact and the stigma of mental illness: a review of the literature. Journal of Mental Health 12:291–305, 2003CrossrefGoogle Scholar

34 Systematic Reviews: CRD’s Guidance for Undertaking Reviews in Health Care. York, United Kingdom, University of York, Centre for Reviews and Dissemination, 2008Google Scholar

35 Hedges LV, Olkin I: Statistical Methods for Meta-Analysis. Orlando, Fla, Academic, 1985Google Scholar

36 Morris SB, DeShon RP: Combining effect size estimates in meta-analysis with repeated measures and independent-groups designs. Psychological Methods 7:105–125, 2002Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

37 Morris SB: Estimating effect sizes from pretest-posttest-control group designs. Organizational Research Methods 11:364–386, 2008CrossrefGoogle Scholar

38 Borenstein M: Effect sizes for continuous data; in The Handbook of Research Synthesis and Meta-Analysis. Edited by Cooper HHedges LVValentine JC. New York, Russell Sage Foundation, 2009Google Scholar

39 Wilson DB: Meta-Analysis Macros for SAS, SPSS, and Stata. Available at mason.gmu.edu/~dwilsonb/ma.html. Accessed Aug 1, 2011Google Scholar

40 Bayar MR, Poyraz BC, Aksoy-Poyraz C, et al.: Reducing mental illness stigma in mental health professionals using a Web-based approach. Israel Journal of Psychiatry and Related Sciences 46:226–230, 2009MedlineGoogle Scholar

41 Borland-Lary D: The effect of an educational program for direct care workers on attitude of stigma toward mentally ill youth in a juvenile justice system. Doctoral dissertation. Baton Rouge, Louisiana State University, Department of Nursing, 2007Google Scholar

42 Brown JF: Faith-based mental health education: a service-learning opportunity for nursing students. Journal of Psychiatric and Mental Health Nursing 16:581–588, 2009Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

43 Brown SA: Implementing a brief hallucination simulation as a mental illness stigma reduction strategy. Community Mental Health Journal 46:500–504, 2010Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

44 Brown SA, Evans Y, Espenschade K, et al.: An examination of two brief stigma reduction strategies: filmed personal contact and hallucination simulations. Community Mental Health Journal 46:494–499, 2010Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

45 Burns A: Reducing stigma: the effect of an educational intervention. Doctoral dissertation. Edmonton, Canada, University of Alberta, Department of Psychology, 2009Google Scholar

46 Campbell NN, Heath J, Bouknight J, et al.: Speaking out for mental health: collaboration of future journalists and psychiatrists. Academic Psychiatry 33:166–168, 2009Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

47 Chan JYN, Mak WWS, Law LSC: Combining education and video-based contact to reduce stigma of mental illness: “The Same or Not the Same” anti-stigma program for secondary schools in Hong Kong. Social Science and Medicine 68:1521–1526, 2009Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

48 Chinnayya HP, Chandrashekar CR, Moily S, et al.: Training primary care health workers in mental health care: evaluation of attitudes towards mental illness before and after training. International Journal of Social Psychiatry 36:300–307, 1990Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

49 Clinton M: Collaborative education and social stereotypes. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Mental Health Nursing 8:100–103, 1999Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

50 Compton MT, Esterberg ML, McGee R, et al.: Crisis intervention team training: changes in knowledge, attitudes, and stigma related to schizophrenia. Psychiatric Services 57:1199–1202, 2006LinkGoogle Scholar

51 Cook JA, Jonikas JA, Razzano L: A randomized evaluation of consumer versus nonconsumer training of state mental health service providers. Community Mental Health Journal 31:229–238, 1995Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

52 Corrigan PW, Larson J, Sells M, et al.: Will filmed presentations of education and contact diminish mental illness stigma? Community Mental Health Journal 43:171–181, 2007Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

53 Corrigan PW, River LP, Lundin RK, et al.: Three strategies for changing attributions about severe mental illness. Schizophrenia Bulletin 27:187–195, 2001Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

54 Corrigan PW, Watson AC, Warpinski AC, et al.: Implications of educating the public on mental illness, violence, and stigma. Psychiatric Services 55:577–580, 2004LinkGoogle Scholar

55 Crisp A, Gelder M, Goddard E, et al.: Stigmatization of people with mental illnesses: a follow-up study within the Changing Minds campaign of the Royal College of Psychiatrists. World Psychiatry 4:106–113, 2005MedlineGoogle Scholar

56 Desocio J, Stember L, Schrinsky J: Teaching children about mental health and illness: a school nurse health education program. Journal of School Nursing 22:81–86, 2006Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

57 Esters IG, Cooker PG, Ittenbach RF: Effects of a unit of instruction in mental health on rural adolescents’ conceptions of mental illness and attitudes about seeking help. Adolescence 33:469–476, 1998MedlineGoogle Scholar

58 Evans ME: Reducing the stigma of mental illness: an analysis of a public education campaign. Doctoral dissertation. Calgary, Alberta, Canada, University of Calgary, Department of Psychology, 2005Google Scholar

59 Faigin DA, Stein CH: Comparing the effects of live and video-taped theatrical performance in decreasing stigmatization of people with serious mental illness. Journal of Mental Health 17:594–606, 2008CrossrefGoogle Scholar

60 Finkelstein J, Lapshin O: Reducing depression stigma using a Web-based program. International Journal of Medical Informatics 76:726–734, 2007Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

61 Gaebel W, Zäske H, Baumann AE, et al.: Evaluation of the German WPA “program against stigma and discrimination because of schizophrenia—Open the Doors”: results from representative telephone surveys before and after three years of antistigma interventions. Schizophrenia Research 98:184–193, 2008Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

62 Godschalx SM: Effect of a mental health educational program upon police officers. Research in Nursing and Health 7:111–117, 1984Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

63 Gould M, Greenberg N, Hetherton J: Stigma and the military: evaluation of a PTSD psychoeducational program. Journal of Traumatic Stress 20:505–515, 2007Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

64 Grausgruber A, Schöny W, Grausgruber-Berner R, et al.: “Schizophrenia has many faces”: evaluation of the Austrian Anti-Stigma Campaign 2000–2002 (in German). Psychiatrische Praxis 36:327–333, 2009Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

65 Griffiths KM, Christensen H, Jorm AF, et al.: Effect of Web-based depression literacy and cognitive-behavioural therapy interventions on stigmatising attitudes to depression: randomised controlled trial. British Journal of Psychiatry 185:342–349, 2004Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

66 Holmes EP, Corrigan PW, Williams P, et al.: Changing attitudes about schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin 25:447–456, 1999Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

67 Hudes S: Multiple factors that affect stigma toward people with schizophrenia: previous knowledge, level of familiarity, and new information. Doctoral dissertation. Los Angeles, Alliant International University, Department of Psychology, 2007Google Scholar

68 Kershenstine T: Stepping into the shoes of schizophrenia: guided imagery as an empathy and anti stigma training tool for mental health provider. Doctoral dissertation. Palo Alto, Calif, Institute of Transpersonal Psychology, Department of Psychology, 2009Google Scholar

69 Lawson-Briddell LY: The efficacy of psycho-educational programs to change the perception of depression and treatment by African American women. Doctoral dissertation. Minneapolis, Minn, Capella University, Department of Psychology, 2006Google Scholar

70 Luty J, Umoh O, Nuamah F: Effect of brief motivational interviewing on stigmatised attitudes towards mental illness. Psychiatric Bulletin 33:212–214, 2009CrossrefGoogle Scholar

71 Luty J, Umoh O, Sessay M, et al.: Effectiveness of Changing Minds campaign factsheets in reducing stigmatised attitudes towards mental illness. Psychiatric Bulletin 31:377–381, 2007CrossrefGoogle Scholar

72 Mann CE, Himelein MJ: Putting the person back into psychopathology: an intervention to reduce mental illness stigma in the classroom. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology 43:545–551, 2008Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

73 Masuda A, Hayes SC, Fletcher LB, et al.: Impact of acceptance and commitment therapy versus education on stigma toward people with psychological disorders. Behaviour Research and Therapy 45:2764–2772, 2007Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

74 McKinney KG: Initial evaluation of Active Minds: a student organization dedicated to reducing the stigma of mental illness. Journal of College Student Psychotherapy 23:281–301, 2009CrossrefGoogle Scholar

75 Mino Y, Yasuda N, Tsuda T, et al.: Effects of a one-hour educational program on medical students’ attitudes to mental illness. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences 55:501–507, 2001Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

76 Molumphy MJ: The stigmatization of the mentally ill by adolescents: the effects of intervention. Doctoral dissertation. New Haven, Southern Connecticut State University, Department of Graduate Studies, 1998Google Scholar

77 Morgan Owusu D: A psycho-educational program about mental illness on the help-seeking attitudes of black college students. Doctoral dissertation. Hempstead, NY, Hofstra University, Department of Psychology, 2003Google Scholar

78 Morrison JK, Becker RE, Bourgeois CA: Decreasing adolescents’ fear of mental patients by means of demythologizing. Psychological Reports 44:855–859, 1979Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

79 Paykel ES, Hart D, Priest RG: Changes in public attitudes to depression during the Defeat Depression campaign. British Journal of Psychiatry 173:519–522, 1998Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

80 Pejović-Milovancević M, Lecić-Tosevski D, Tenjović L, et al.: Changing attitudes of high school students towards peers with mental health problems. Psychiatria Danubina 21:213–219, 2009MedlineGoogle Scholar

81 Penn DL, Corrigan PW: The effects of stereotype suppression on psychiatric stigma. Schizophrenia Research 55:269–276, 2002Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

82 Petchers MK, Biegel DE, Drescher R: A video-based program to educate high school students about serious mental illness. Hospital and Community Psychiatry 39:1102–1103, 1988AbstractGoogle Scholar

83 Pinfold V, Huxley P, Thornicroft G, et al.: Reducing psychiatric stigma and discrimination—evaluating an educational intervention with the police force in England. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology 38:337–344, 2003Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

84 Pinfold V, Thornicroft G, Huxley P, et al.: Active ingredients in anti-stigma programmes in mental health. International Review of Psychiatry 17:123–131, 2005Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

85 Pinfold V, Toulmin H, Thornicroft G, et al.: Reducing psychiatric stigma and discrimination: evaluation of educational interventions in UK secondary schools. British Journal of Psychiatry 182:342–346, 2003Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

86 Pittman JO, Noh S, Coleman D: Evaluating the effectiveness of a consumer delivered anti-stigma program: replication with graduate-level helping professionals. Psychiatric Rehabilitation Journal 33:236–238, 2010Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

87 Rafacz JR: Anti-stigma programs: stigma in campus police officers. Doctoral dissertation. Chicago, Illinois Institute of Technology, Department of Psychology, 2010Google Scholar

88 Rickwood D, Cavanagh S, Curtis L, et al.: Educating young people about mental health and mental illness: evaluating a school-based programme. International Journal of Mental Health Promotion 6:23–32, 2004CrossrefGoogle Scholar

89 Ritterfeld U, Jin SA: Addressing media stigma for people experiencing mental illness using an entertainment-education strategy. Journal of Health Psychology 11:247–267, 2006Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

90 Roberts G, Somers J, Dawe J, et al.: On the edge: a drama-based mental health education programme on early psychosis for schools. Early Intervention in Psychiatry 1:168–176, 2007CrossrefGoogle Scholar

91 Roberts LM, Wiskin C, Roalfe A: Effects of exposure to mental illness in role-play on undergraduate student attitudes. Family Medicine 40:477–483, 2008MedlineGoogle Scholar

92 Rusch L: Depression stigma reduction: the impact of models of depression on stigma and treatment seeking. Doctoral dissertation. University of Wisconsin–Milwaukee, Department of Psychology, 2010Google Scholar

93 Rusch LC, Kanter JW, Angelone AF, et al.: The impact of In Our Own Voice on stigma. American Journal of Psychiatric Rehabilitation 11:373–389, 2008Google Scholar

94 Rusch LC, Kanter JW, Brondino MJ: A comparison of contextual and biomedical models of stigma reduction for depression with a nonclinical undergraduate sample. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 197:104–110, 2009Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

95 Sadow D, Ryder M: Reducing stigmatizing attitudes held by future health professionals: the person is the message. Psychological Services 5:362–372, 2008CrossrefGoogle Scholar

96 Saporito JM: Reducing stigma toward seeking mental health treatment. Doctoral dissertation. Charlottesville, University of Virginia, Department of Psychology, 2009Google Scholar

97 Schmetzer AD, Lafuze JE, Jack ME: Overcoming stigma: involving families in medical student and psychiatric residency education. Academic Psychiatry 32:127–131, 2008Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

98 Schulze B, Richter-Werling M, Matschinger H, et al.: Crazy? So what! Effects of a school project on students’ attitudes towards people with schizophrenia. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica 107:142–150, 2003Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

99 Sharp W: Help-seeking and mental health education: an evaluation of a classroom-based strategy to modify help-seeking for mental health problems. Doctoral dissertation. Lafayette, University of Mississippi, Department of Psychology, 2007Google Scholar

100 Shera W, Delva-Tauiliili J: Changing MSW students’ attitudes towards the severely mentally ill. Community Mental Health Journal 32:159–169, 1996Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

101 Shor R, Sykes IJ: Introducing structured dialogue with people with mental illness into the training of social work students. Psychiatric Rehabilitation Journal 26:63–69, 2002Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

102 Spagnolo AB: Examining the effect of anti-stigma messages on the attitudes of early adolescents. Doctoral dissertation. Newark, University of Medicine and Dentistry of New Jersey, Department of Psychology, 2009Google Scholar

103 Spagnolo AB, Murphy AA, Librera LA: Reducing stigma by meeting and learning from people with mental illness. Psychiatric Rehabilitation Journal 31:186–193, 2008Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

104 Stuart H: Reaching out to high school youth: the effectiveness of a video-based antistigma program. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, Revue canadienne de psychiatrie 51:647–653, 2006Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

105 Swartz KL, Kastelic EA, Hess SG, et al.: The effectiveness of a school-based adolescent depression education program. Health Education and Behavior 37:11–22, 2010Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

106 Tanaka G, Ogawa T, Inadomi H, et al.: Effects of an educational program on public attitudes towards mental illness. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences 57:595–602, 2003Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

107 Watson AC, Otey E, Westbrook AL, et al.: Changing middle schoolers’ attitudes about mental illness through education. Schizophrenia Bulletin 30:563–572, 2004Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

108 Worakul P, Thavichachart N, Lueboonthavatchai P: Effects of psycho-educational program on knowledge and attitude upon schizophrenia of schizophrenic patients’ caregivers. Journal of the Medical Association of Thailand 90:1199–1204, 2007MedlineGoogle Scholar

109 Wundsam K, Pitschel-Walz G, Leucht S, et al.: Psychiatric patients and relatives instruct German police officers: an anti-stigma project of “BASTA—The Alliance for Mentally Ill People (in German). Psychiatrische Praxis 34:181–187, 2007Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

110 Corrigan P, Roe D, Tsang H: Challenging the Stigma of Mental Illness: Lessons for Therapists and Advocates. West Sussex, United Kingdom, Wiley-Blackwell, 2011Google Scholar

111 Borman G, Grigg J: Visual and narrative interpretation; in The Handbook of Research Synthesis and Meta-Analysis. Edited by Cooper HHedges LVValentine JC. New York, Russell Sage Foundation, 2009Google Scholar

112 Cohen J: A power primer. Psychological Bulletin 112:155–159, 1992Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

113 Valentine JC: Judging the quality of primary research; in The Handbook of Research Synthesis and Meta-Analysis. Edited by Cooper HHedges LVValentine JC. New York, Russell Sage Foundation, 2009Google Scholar